Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 21 |
Author
Bakeman, Roger | 1 |
Bearison, David J. | 1 |
BenDebba, Mohamed | 1 |
Bereiter, Carl | 1 |
Blair, Clancy B. | 1 |
Brown, Penelope | 1 |
Brownell, Celia A. | 1 |
Brownlee, John R. | 1 |
Burtenshaw, Tiarn | 1 |
Casillas, Marisa | 1 |
Cheskes, Jaime | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 14 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 4 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lister, Casey J.; Burtenshaw, Tiarn; Walker, Bradley; Ohan, Jeneva L.; Fay, Nicolas – Child Development, 2021
Naturalistic studies show that children can create language-like communication systems in the absence of conventional language. However, experimental evidence is mixed. We address this discrepancy using an experimental paradigm that simulates naturalistic sign creation. Specifically, we tested if a sample of 6- to 12-year-old children (52 girls…
Descriptors: Case Studies, Sign Language, Nonverbal Communication, Comparative Analysis
Cristia, Alejandrina; Dupoux, Emmanuel; Gurven, Michael; Stieglitz, Jonathan – Child Development, 2019
This article provides an estimation of how frequently, and from whom, children aged 0-11 years (Ns between 9 and 24) receive one-on-one verbal input among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists of lowland Bolivia. Analyses of systematic daytime behavioral observations reveal < 1 min per daylight hour is spent talking to children younger than 4 years…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Children, Verbal Communication, Indigenous Populations
Casillas, Marisa; Brown, Penelope; Levinson, Stephen C. – Child Development, 2020
Daylong at-home audio recordings from 10 Tseltal Mayan children (0;2-3;0; Southern Mexico) were analyzed for how often children engaged in verbal interaction with others and whether their speech environment changed with age, time of day, household size, and number of speakers present. Children were infrequently directly spoken to, with most…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Verbal Communication, Interaction, Speech Communication
Kuhn, Laura J.; Willoughby, Michael T.; Wilbourn, Makeba Parramore; Vernon-Feagans, Lynne; Blair, Clancy B. – Child Development, 2014
Using an epidemiological sample (N = 1,117) and a prospective longitudinal design, this study tested the direct and indirect effects of preverbal and verbal communication (15 months to 3 years) on executive function (EF) at age 4 years. Results indicated that whereas gestures (15 months), as well as language (2 and 3 years), were correlated with…
Descriptors: Epidemiology, Nonverbal Communication, Longitudinal Studies, Verbal Communication

Rebelsky, Freda; Hanks, Cheryl – Child Development, 1971
The data from this study shows that fathers spend little time vocalizing to their infants and that the number of interactions varies by time of day, age and sex of infant and the kind of activity occurring during the interaction. (Author/WY)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Fathers, Infants, Parent Role

Lougee, Michael D.; And Others – Child Development, 1977
Results indicate that normative data derived from observations of same-age interaction may not be generalized to the mixed-age situation and that the two types of social experience may serve different functions in children's development. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Preschool Children, Preschool Education, Research

Bearison, David J.; Levey, Linda M. – Child Development, 1977
A sample of 90 children in kindergarten, second, and fourth grades were presented with ambiguous referential communication messages and asked to judge the quality of the messages. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Communication Skills, Comprehension, Elementary Education

Wimmer, Heinz – Child Development, 1988
A sharp improvement in children's understanding of the role of visual perception and linguistic communication in knowledge functions was found between the ages of three and five years. (PCB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Epistemology, Foreign Countries

Bereiter, Carl; And Others – Child Development, 1979
Reports three experiments based on the hypothesis that qualitative changes in verbal reasoning emerge, not from the conclusions children draw, but from what they accept as conclusive evidence. Results show a gradual development across 7-13 age range in ability to distinguish logically certain from only suggested or probabilistic conclusions.…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Age Differences, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students

Meyer, Jerome S. – Child Development, 1978
Descriptors: Age Differences, Elementary School Students, Memory, Pictorial Stimuli

Brownell, Celia A. – Child Development, 1988
Children's ability to produce integrated sequences of discrete behaviors was examined as a function of age and task demands for several behavioral domains. Results are discussed in terms of possible age-related constraints on combinatorial skills that operate at a general, cross-domain level during toddlerhood. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Patterns, Cognitive Ability, Performance Factors

Howes, Carollee – Child Development, 1985
Social play emerged earlier than social pretend play with a similar structure; the incidence of social pretend play increased with age. Four strategies for integrating pretense into social play were isolated; among them, verbal recruitment and "join" were found to be more effective than imitation or nonverbal recruitment. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cooperation, Imitation, Incidence

Feldman, Nina S.; Ruble, Diane N. – Child Development, 1988
Examines personal relevance in relation to the ability of 48 5- to 10-year-old children to describe others in terms of psychological characteristics. Results suggest that verbal inferencing skills of young children have been underestimated and that they may be more oriented toward affective goals than older children when anticipating interaction…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Inferences, Motivation, Peer Relationship

El-Sheikh, Mona; Cheskes, Jaime – Child Development, 1995
Examined responses of 6- to 7- and 9 to 10-year olds to adult-child conflict, compared them to responses to adult-adult conflict, and further examined responses in relation to verbal and physical expression of anger. Participants in the conflict, the form of anger, and the age and gender of the subject moderated some responses. (DR)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Aggression, Anger, Conflict Resolution

Garvey, Catherine; BenDebba, Mohamed – Child Development, 1974
Descriptors: Age Differences, Interaction Process Analysis, Peer Relationship, Preschool Children
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2