Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Classification | 6 |
Visual Discrimination | 6 |
Visual Stimuli | 4 |
Infants | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Color | 2 |
Novelty (Stimulus Dimension) | 2 |
Preschool Children | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Behavior Theories | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Bauer, Patricia J. | 1 |
Catherwood, Di | 1 |
Deak, Gedeon O. | 1 |
Ebeling, Karen S. | 1 |
Gelman, Susan A. | 1 |
Johnson, Kathy E. | 1 |
Munakata, Yuko | 1 |
Quinn, Paul C. | 1 |
Yerys, Benjamin E. | 1 |
Younger, Barbara A. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Younger, Barbara A.; Johnson, Kathy E. – Child Development, 2006
Previous research suggests that model competence does not emerge until relatively late in infancy (20-26 months). Development was systematically analyzed within 3 key areas--count noun learning, dual representation, and categorization--hypothesized to support the emergence of model competence in the second year. In an object-handling preferential…
Descriptors: Infants, Models, Concept Formation, Visual Discrimination

Deak, Gedeon O.; Bauer, Patricia J. – Child Development, 1996
Three experiments explored effects of stimulus and task factors on tendency to categorize according to taxonomic relations when those relations conflict with appearances. When provided with information that constrained categorization, preschoolers and adults reliably based their decisions on taxonomic relations between physically dissimilar items.…
Descriptors: Adults, Classification, Performance Factors, Preschool Children

Quinn, Paul C. – Child Development, 1994
Three experiments using the familiarization-novelty preference procedure confirmed the hypothesis that three-month-old infants could form categorical representations of spatial relations above and below. The infants, after being shown a familiarization diagram with a dot appearing in multiple locations below a line, showed a preference for a novel…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Infants, Spatial Ability

Catherwood, Di; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Confirms that infants, like older children, are capable of responding categorically to stimuli of different shapes if these are similar in hue. (PCB)
Descriptors: Classification, Color, Infants, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)

Ebeling, Karen S.; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 1994
Three experiments examined how flexibly two- to four-year-old children use the words "big" and "little" in normative, perceptual, and functional contexts. Results showed that children switched easily from a normative context but made errors when asked to switch to a normative context. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Classification, Context Effect
Yerys, Benjamin E.; Munakata, Yuko – Child Development, 2006
Children often perseverate, repeating prior behaviors when inappropriate. This work tested the roles of verbal labels and stimulus novelty in such perseveration. Three-year-old children sorted cards by one rule and were then instructed to switch to a second rule. In a basic condition, cards had familiar shapes and colors and both rules were stated…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Persistence, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes