NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 12 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ju, Narae; Williams, Natalie; Sedivy, Julie; Chambers, Craig G.; Graham, Susan A. – Child Development, 2023
This study examined 4- and 5-year-olds' incremental interpretation of size adjectives, focusing on whether contrastive inferences are modulated by speaker behavior. Children (N = 120, 59 females, mostly White, tested between July, 2018 and August, 2019) encountered either a conventional or unconventional speaker who labeled objects in a…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Inferences, Interpersonal Communication, Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schulze, Cornelia; Grassmann, Susanne; Tomasello, Michael – Child Development, 2013
Three studies investigated 3-year-old children's ability to determine a speaker's communicative intent when the speaker's overt utterance related to that intent only indirectly. Studies 1 and 2 examined children's comprehension of indirectly stated requests (e.g., "I find Xs good" can imply, in context, a request for…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Inferences, Verbal Communication, Intention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Miele, David B.; Son, Lisa K.; Metcalfe, Janet – Child Development, 2013
Recent studies have shown that the metacognitive judgments adults infer from their experiences of encoding effort vary in accordance with their naive theories of intelligence. To determine whether this finding extends to elementary schoolchildren, a study was conducted in which 27 third graders (M[subscript age] = 8.27) and 24 fifth graders…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Evaluative Thinking, Intelligence, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Filippova, Eva; Astington, Janet Wilde – Child Development, 2008
This study describes the development of social reasoning in school-age children. An irony task is used to assess 5-, 7-, and 9-year-olds' (N = 72) and adults' (N = 24) recursive understanding of others' minds. Guttman scale analysis demonstrates that in order to understand a speaker's communicative intention, a child needs to recognize the…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Language Aptitude, Cognitive Development, Social Cognition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smith, Robin; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Despite assertions to the contrary, preschool children are capable of understanding cinematic events conveyed through camera techniques and film editing. This ability nevertheless substantially increases with age among children from four- to seven-years-old. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Films
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fabricius, William V.; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Examined concepts of mental activities involved in acts of knowing in 54 children of 8-10 years and adults. Ten-year-olds and adults judged memory involvement to be the most important relation among mental activities. Eight-year-olds judged comprehension and attention according to the involvement of visual or verbal information. (RJC)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Attention, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Flavell, John H.; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Results of four studies confirmed the hypothesis that three year olds would have less difficulty inferring that another person holds an odd belief about a matter of taste or value than they have in inferring that another person holds a false belief about a matter of verifiable fact. (RH)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moses, Louis J.; Flavell, John H. – Child Development, 1990
Two experiments investigated the possibility that three year olds would do better on tasks in which belief cues were stronger than on standard false belief tasks, in which the children could reason backward to the belief from its effects. Findings provided strong support for the view that three year olds do not fully understand the…
Descriptors: Behavior, Beliefs, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Springer, Ken – Child Development, 1995
Two experiments studied how preschool children acquire a naive theory of kinship (NTK). Overall, results implicate a type of theory building that involves inferences from preexisting knowledge rather than structural change, use of analogy, or acquisition of new knowledge. (DR)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Concept Formation, Family Relationship, Family Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Beal, Carole R. – Child Development, 1990
Four studies determined when first, second, and third graders recognize that they make inferences to understand text, and the effect of this recognition on their ability to revise text and monitor its informativeness. Younger children tended to attribute inferred information to the text, while older children clearly distinguished inferred and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Difficulty Level
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gnepp, Jackie; Gould, Martha E. – Child Development, 1985
Among kindergarten children, second-, fifth-grade, and college students, a gradual age-related increase was found in the ability to interpret an event from another person's perspective in light of that person's prior experiences. Various alternative explanations for the developmental trend were evaluated with data from subjects receiving prompts…
Descriptors: Age Differences, College Students, Comprehension, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ackerman, Brian P. – Child Development, 1988
Experiments revealed that children seemed able to integrate multiple sources of information but were more dependent on clue support and generally less likely to infer reason than adults. Children were more likely than adults to reject premise as an explanation of outcome. Only fourth-graders and adults modified inferences in response to resolution…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension