Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Source
Child Development | 30 |
Author
Nicholls, John G. | 2 |
Braine, Lila G. | 1 |
Bryant, Joan D. | 1 |
Bryant, Peter | 1 |
Casasola, Marianella | 1 |
Childers, Perry | 1 |
Coley, John D. | 1 |
Compton, Donald L. | 1 |
Estes, David | 1 |
Feldman, David, Henry | 1 |
Fisher, Celia B. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 23 |
Reports - Research | 22 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 7 |
Location
Austria | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pruden, Shannon M.; Roseberry, Sarah; Goksun, Tilbe; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
Fundamental to amassing a lexicon of relational terms (i.e., verbs, prepositions) is the ability to abstract and categorize spatial relations such as a figure (e.g., "boy") moving along a path (e.g., "around" the barn). Three studies examine how infants learn to categorize path over changes in "manner," or how an action is performed (e.g., running…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, English, Language Acquisition
Low, Jason; Simpson, Samantha – Child Development, 2012
Executive function mechanisms underpinning language-related effects on theory of mind understanding were examined in a sample of 165 preschoolers. Verbal labels were manipulated to identify relevant perspectives on an explicit false belief task. In Experiment 1 with 4-year-olds (N = 74), false belief reasoning was superior in the fully and…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Preschool Children, Executive Function, Beliefs
Fuchs, Lynn S.; Geary, David C.; Compton, Donald L.; Fuchs, Douglas; Hamlett, Carol L.; Bryant, Joan D. – Child Development, 2010
Contributions of domain-general and domain-specific numerical competencies were assessed on first graders' number combination skill (NC) and word-problem skill (WP). Students (n = 205) between 5 and 7 years of age were assessed on 2 aspects of numerosity, 8 domain-general abilities, NC, and WP. Both aspects of numerosity predicted NC when…
Descriptors: Numeracy, School Readiness, Short Term Memory, Concept Formation

White, Kathleen M. – Child Development, 1971
Descriptors: Ability Identification, Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Ability

Fisher, Celia B.; Braine, Lila G. – Child Development, 1981
Found that preschool children can form abstract concepts of left and right which are not bound to the specific training context: children were able to generalize to new figures and to new spatial locations. The nature of the preschool child's left-right judgments is discussed. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Discrimination Learning, Generalization, Preschool Children

Pillow, Bradford H.; Flavell, John H. – Child Development, 1986
Four experiments investigated three- and four-year-old children's knowledge of projective size-distance and projective shape-orientation relationships. Results indicated that preschool children's understanding of these relationships seems at least partly cognitive rather than wholly perceptive, providing further evidence for the acquisition of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Concept Formation, Preschool Children, Spatial Ability

Childers, Perry; Wimmer, Mary – Child Development, 1971
Evidence indicated that the awareness of death as universal is a function of age; the understanding of death as irrevocable was not demonstrated systematically through age 10. (Authors)
Descriptors: Age, Cognitive Ability, Concept Formation, Data Analysis

Coley, John D.; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 1989
Investigated the interpretation of the word "big" by 40 children of 3 to 5 years. The type and orientation of objects used in the study were varied. Results demonstrated that contextual factors influenced children's responses. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition

Pratt, Chris; Bryant, Peter – Child Development, 1990
Results of three experiments suggest that, in contrast to the claims made by Wimmer and others (1988), three- and four-year-old children understand that looking leads to knowing. The three- and four-year-olds' difficulty in the study lay mainly in the form of the questions that they were asked. (RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Concept Formation, Foreign Countries

Hazen, Nancy L.; And Others – Child Development, 1978
Children aged three to six years were taught a specified route through an environment and were tested on their ability to: (1) travel the route in reverse, (2) name the sequence of landmarks along the reverse route, (3) infer the relationship between parts of the environment not directly traveled between, and (4) construct a model of the…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Concept Formation, Developmental Stages

Lyons-Ruth, Karlen – Child Development, 1981
Examines young children's awareness that an actor's intention to transfer possession or control is a necessary precondition for correct use of the verb "give". Ninety-six four- through six-year-olds were presented with anecdotes in which the actor's behavior was held constant but his subjective goal was varied. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Concept Formation
Casasola, Marianella – Child Development, 2005
Two experiments explored how infants learn to form an abstract categorical representation of support (i.e., on) when habituated to few (i.e., 2) or many (i.e., 6) examples of the relation. When habituated to 2 pairs of objects in a support relation, 14-month-olds, but not 10-month-olds, formed the abstract spatial category (i.e., generalized the…
Descriptors: Infants, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Classification, Habituation

Oviatt, Sharon L. – Child Development, 1982
Examines the development of infants' ability to begin recognizing novel referents of common object names. In particular, the present experiment investigated the development of 12- to 20-month-old infants' ability to infer that an unfamiliar but categorically related object can be designated by a newly learned name for the object class. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation

Keating, Daniel P.; And Others – Child Development, 1980
Examines the role of basic cognitive-processing efficiency as the source of developmental variance in cognitive performance. Two experimental tasks, memory and visual scanning, were used to investigate age effects on the search-processing parameter. Subjects were 9-, 11-, 13-, and 15-year-old children. (CM)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Ability

Nicholls, John G. – Child Development, 1978
Selected cognitive developments presumed to mediate the development of achievement motivation are described. Age trends for four causal schemes involving the concepts of effort and ability from 5 to 13 years of age are presented. Developments related to ability, task difficulty, and incentive value are also described. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Ability, Age Differences, Attribution Theory, Concept Formation
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2