Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Cues | 42 |
Memory | 36 |
Age Differences | 25 |
Recall (Psychology) | 20 |
Preschool Children | 12 |
Elementary School Students | 11 |
Cognitive Processes | 9 |
Infants | 9 |
Children | 6 |
Research | 6 |
Child Development | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 42 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 26 |
Reports - Research | 26 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
California | 1 |
China | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Woodcock Johnson Tests of… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Jensen, Toril S.; Berntsen, Dorthe; Kingo, Osman S.; Krøjgaard, Peter – Child Development, 2022
Verbally reported long-term memory for past events typically improves with age. However, such findings are based exclusively on studies, where children are directly asked to recall. The present study showed that when 3- (n = 113, 59 girls) and 4-year-olds (n = 113, 62 girls), predominantly White, were brought back to a distinct laboratory-setting…
Descriptors: Environmental Influences, Cues, Recall (Psychology), Early Experience
Guo, Dong; Wang, Yudan; Liao, Yifan; Li, Jiaofeng; Zhang, Xingyi; Gao, Zaifeng; Shen, Mowei; He, Jie – Child Development, 2022
Visual working memory (WM) plays a pivotal role in integrating fragments into meaningful units, but no study has addressed how visual WM integration takes place in children. The current study examined whether WM integration emerges once preschoolers master Gestalt cue and can retain two representations in WM (automatic integration hypothesis), or…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Age Differences, Cues
Redshaw, Jonathan; Vandersee, Johanna; Bulley, Adam; Gilbert, Sam J. – Child Development, 2018
This study explored under what conditions young children would set reminders to aid their memory for delayed intentions. A computerized task requiring participants to carry out delayed intentions under varying levels of cognitive load was presented to 63 children (aged between 6.9 and 13.0 years old). Children of all ages demonstrated…
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Intention, Prompting
Clegg, Jennifer M.; Legare, Cristine H. – Child Development, 2016
Four tasks (N = 191, 3- to 6-year-olds) examined the effect of instrumental versus conventional language cues on children's imitative fidelity of a necklace-making activity, their memory and transmission of the activity, and their perceptions of functional fixedness. Children in the conventional condition imitated with higher fidelity, transmitted…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Cues, Task Analysis, Imitation
Khu, Melanie; Chambers, Craig; Graham, Susan A. – Child Development, 2018
Using a novel emotional perspective-taking task, this study investigated 4-year-olds' (n = 97) use of a speaker's emotional prosody to make inferences about the speaker's emotional state and, correspondingly, their communicative intent. Eye gaze measures indicated preschoolers used emotional perspective inferences to guide their real-time…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Child Development, Intonation, Suprasegmentals
Shimi, Andria; Nobre, Anna C.; Astle, Duncan; Scerif, Gaia – Child Development, 2014
How does developing attentional control operate within visual short-term memory (VSTM)? Seven-year-olds, 11-year-olds, and adults (total n = 205) were asked to report whether probe items were part of preceding visual arrays. In Experiment 1, central or peripheral cues oriented attention to the location of to-be-probed items either prior to…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Short Term Memory, Children, Adults
Barr, Rachel; Walker, Joanne; Gross, Julien; Hayne, Harlene – Child Development, 2014
The concept of spreading activation describes how retrieval of one memory cues retrieval of other memories that are associated with it. This study explored spreading activation in 6-, 12-, and 18-month-old infants. Infants (n = 144) learned two tasks within the same experimental session; one task, deferred imitation (DI), is typically remembered…
Descriptors: Infants, Age Differences, Memory, Cues
Phillips, Brenda; Seston, Rebecca; Kelemen, Deborah – Child Development, 2012
Prior research has found that toddlers will form enduring artifact categories after direct exposure to an adult using a novel tool. Four studies explored whether 2- (N = 48) and 3-year-olds (N = 32) demonstrate this same capacity when learning by eavesdropping. After surreptitiously observing an adult use 1 of 2 artifacts to operate a bell via a…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Adults, Familiarity, Observational Learning
Davis, Elizabeth L.; Levine, Linda J. – Child Development, 2013
The link between emotion regulation and academic achievement is well documented. Less is known about specific emotion regulation strategies that promote learning. Six- to 13-year-olds ("N" = 126) viewed a sad film and were instructed to reappraise the importance, reappraise the outcome, or ruminate about the sad events; another group…
Descriptors: Child Development, Memory, Self Control, Emotional Response
Peterson, Carole; Warren, Kelly L.; Short, Megan M. – Child Development, 2011
Although infantile amnesia has been investigated for many years in adults, only recently has it been investigated in children. This study was a 2-year follow-up and extension of an earlier study. Children (4-13 years old) were asked initially and 2 years later for their earliest 3 memories. At follow-up, their age at the time of these memories…
Descriptors: Cues, Comparative Analysis, Memory, Children
Odegard, Timothy N.; Cooper, Crystal M.; Lampinen, James M.; Reyna, Valerie F.; Brainerd, Charles J. – Child Development, 2009
The present research examined the influence of prior knowledge on children's free recall, cued recall, recognition memory, and source memory judgments for a series of similar real-life events. Forty children (5-12 years old) attended 4 thematic birthday parties and were later interviewed about the events that transpired during the parties using…
Descriptors: Child Development, Memory, Prior Learning, Recognition (Psychology)

Horn, Hilary A.; Myers, Nancy Angrist – Child Development, 1978
Describes three delayed-response experiments which tested two- and three-year-old children's memories for location of a hidden object under several combinations of spatial and pictorial cue availability and emphasis. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Pictorial Stimuli, Preschool Children

Ratner, Hilary Horn; Myers, Nancy Angrist – Child Development, 1980
Two-year-old children's memory for locations of hidden objects was examined in four cue conditions. Pictures marked hidden-object locations in three of these conditions, and either depicted or were related associatively to hidden objects. In the fourth condition, only blank cards were presented with the objects. (Author/SS)
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Influences, Memory

Bartlett, James C.; Santrock, John W. – Child Development, 1979
Reports an experiment with five-year-old children which tested the hypothesis that a change in affect between input and test interferes with performance in a nominally noncued free recall test but not with performance on a cued recall test. (JMB)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Cues, Memory, Preschool Children

Geis, Mary Fulcher; Lange, Garrett – Child Development, 1976
First, third, and fifth graders hid a series of "people pictures" in containers, half of which bore picture cues related to the people's societal roles and half of which bore cues unrelated to the people's roles. Aspects of their memory-for-location were then analyzed by grade level. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cues, Elementary School Students, Memory