Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Classification | 16 |
Developmental Stages | 16 |
Children | 7 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Child Development | 5 |
Young Children | 5 |
Concept Formation | 4 |
Visual Stimuli | 4 |
Cognitive Development | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Animals | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 16 |
Author
Taylor, Marianne G. | 2 |
Baldwin, Dare A. | 1 |
Bjorklund, David F. | 1 |
Bloom, Paul | 1 |
Boom, Jan | 1 |
Breslow, Leonard | 1 |
Brugman, Daniel | 1 |
Casasola, Marianella | 1 |
Cowan, Philip A. | 1 |
Demorest, Amy P. | 1 |
Diesendruck, Gil | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Translations | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 3 |
Location
Netherlands | 1 |
Russia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Wetzel, Nicole; Scharf, Florian; Widmann, Andreas – Child Development, 2019
Attention control abilities are relevant for learning success. Little is known about the development of audio-visual attention in early childhood. Four groups of children between the ages of 4 and 10 years and adults performed an audio-visual distraction paradigm (N = 106). Multilevel analyses revealed increased reaction times in a visual…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Auditory Stimuli, Visual Stimuli, Task Analysis
Casasola, Marianella; Park, Youjeong – Child Development, 2013
Two experiments examined infants' ability to form a spatial category when habituated to few (only 2) or many (6) exemplars of a spatial relation. Sixty-four infants of 10 months and 64 infants of 14 months were habituated to dynamic events in which a toy was placed in a consistent spatial relation ("in" or "on") to a referent…
Descriptors: Infants, Spatial Ability, Classification, Child Development
Margett, Tessa E.; Witherington, David C. – Child Development, 2011
This study investigated preschoolers' living kinds conceptualization by employing an extensive stimulus set and alternate indices of understanding. Thirty-four 3- to 5-year-olds and 36 adult undergraduates completed 3 testing phases involving 4 object classes: plants, animals, mobile, and immobile artifacts. The phases involved inquiries…
Descriptors: Testing, Preschool Children, Undergraduate Students, Biology
Taylor, Marianne G.; Rhodes, Marjorie; Gelman, Susan A. – Child Development, 2009
Two studies (N = 456) compared the development of concepts of animal species and human gender, using a switched-at-birth reasoning task. Younger children (5- and 6-year-olds) treated animal species and human gender as equivalent; they made similar levels of category-based inferences and endorsed similar explanations for development in these 2…
Descriptors: Animals, Classification, Environmental Influences, Inferences

Winer, Gerald A. – Child Development, 1980
Examines the relationship between class inclusion and age and indicates that class inclusion frequently appears to develop at a much later age than is suggested in Piaget's writings. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Children, Classification, Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages

Garber, Judy – Child Development, 1984
Provides a developmental framework for the classification of psychopathology in children and highlights the contributions that such classifications may have toward the understanding of normal development. Specific attention is given to the concepts of continuity and normality and their implications for the manner in which developmental…
Descriptors: Adjustment (to Environment), Child Development, Children, Classification

Breslow, Leonard; Cowan, Philip A. – Child Development, 1984
A total of 14 psychotic children with a mean age of nine years, two months, and 14 normal children having a mean age of six years, four months, were compared in terms of structural level and functional abilities on classification and seriation tasks. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Children, Classification, Comparative Analysis, Developmental Stages

Baldwin, Dare A. – Child Development, 1989
Expectations concerning form and color in object label referencing of 80 children of 2-3 years were examined in 2 studies. Findings show that children as young as 2 expect form similarity to be a better guide than color similarity to the extension of object labels. (RJC)
Descriptors: Classification, Color, Developmental Stages, Learning Processes

Selman, Robert L.; Demorest, Amy P. – Child Development, 1984
A transcript-narrative analysis technique was used to identify interpersonal negotiation strategies of two nine-year-old boys selected from a pool of children with socioemotional and interpersonal difficulties. Strategies were classified according to four developmental levels: impulsive/physical, unilateral/coercive, reciprocal/influential, and…
Descriptors: Children, Classification, Developmental Stages, Emotional Problems

Merriman, William E. – Child Development, 1986
Evaluates some possible reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors in an experiment in which two-, four-, and six-year-olds learned two artificial object names in succession. (HOD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Classification, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development

Lillard, Angeline S. – Child Development, 1996
Five experiments investigated whether children, ages three to eight, think of pretending as a mental state. Results indicated that most children under six see pretending as primarily physical. Eight-year-olds claimed that execution of pretense did not involve the mind, although the planning aspect of pretense did. (MOK)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Ability, Concept Formation

Diesendruck, Gil; Bloom, Paul – Child Development, 2003
Three studies explored whether children's tendency to extend object names on the basis of sameness of shape (shape bias) is specific to naming. Findings indicated that 2- and 3-year-olds showed shape bias both when asked to extend a novel name and when asked to select an object of the same kind as a target object; 3-year-olds also showed shape…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Beliefs, Bias, Classification

Bjorklund, David F.; de Marchena, Melanie R. – Child Development, 1984
Reports two experiments showing a possible developmental shift from memory organization based on associative criteria to an organization based on categorical criteria. Children in first, fourth, and seventh grades were given a sort/recall task with items that could be organized into groups of categorical or associative pairs. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Children, Classification, Cluster Analysis

Taylor, Marianne G. – Child Development, 1996
Examined children's beliefs about the origins of gender differences and age-related changes in these beliefs. Findings suggested that young children may have an early bias to view gender categories as predictive of essential, underlying similarities between members but later come to acknowledge the role of other causal mechanisms in shaping how…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Beliefs, Biological Influences, Childhood Attitudes

Boom, Jan; Brugman, Daniel; van der Heijden, Peter G. M. – Child Development, 2001
Asked Dutch university and Russian high school students to sort statements in terms of moral sophistication to investigate hierarchical stage structure of moral stages. Found that sorting statements representative of stages below one's own was straightforward; sorting statements above one's stage was difficult, suggesting that reflective…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Classification, College Students, Developmental Stages
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2