Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Familiarity | 6 |
Memory | 4 |
Recognition (Psychology) | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Recall (Psychology) | 3 |
Children | 2 |
Experiments | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
Short Term Memory | 2 |
Young Children | 2 |
Adults | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Bauer, Patricia J. | 1 |
Catherine Mimeau | 1 |
Geurten, Marie | 1 |
Hertsgaard, Louise A. | 1 |
Holliday, Robyn E. | 1 |
Kelemen, Deborah | 1 |
Lloyd, Marianne | 1 |
Mei Zhou | 1 |
Nelson, Charles A. | 1 |
Phillips, Brenda | 1 |
Puyuan Zhang | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
China | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Mei Zhou; Puyuan Zhang; Catherine Mimeau; Shelley Xiuli Tong – Child Development, 2024
Abstract The relation between statistical learning and working memory in children with developmental dyslexia (DD) remains unclear. This study employed a distributional and a conditional statistical learning experiment and a working memory task to examine this relation in 651 Chinese 6- to 12-year-olds with and without DD (N[subscript DD] = 199,…
Descriptors: Statistics Education, Short Term Memory, Foreign Countries, Children
Geurten, Marie; Willems, Sylvie; Lloyd, Marianne – Child Development, 2021
We tested whether changes in attribution processes could account for the developmental differences observed in how children's use fluency to guide their memory decisions. Children ranging in age from 4 to 9 years studied a list of familiar or unfamiliar cartoon characters. In Experiment 1 (n = 84), participants completed a recognition test during…
Descriptors: Young Children, Attribution Theory, Memory, Recognition (Psychology)
Phillips, Brenda; Seston, Rebecca; Kelemen, Deborah – Child Development, 2012
Prior research has found that toddlers will form enduring artifact categories after direct exposure to an adult using a novel tool. Four studies explored whether 2- (N = 48) and 3-year-olds (N = 32) demonstrate this same capacity when learning by eavesdropping. After surreptitiously observing an adult use 1 of 2 artifacts to operate a bell via a…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Adults, Familiarity, Observational Learning

Bauer, Patricia J.; Hertsgaard, Louise A. – Child Development, 1993
Results of 3 experiments indicated that 13.5- and 16.5-month-old children recalled multiple sequences after a 1-week delay. Without cues, the recall of 16.5-month olds was facilitated by familiarity and by enabling relations; only enabling relations aided the 13.5-month olds' recall. With verbal cues, the recall of 13.5- and 16.5-month olds was…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Familiarity, Infants, Long Term Memory

de Haan, Michelle; Nelson, Charles A. – Child Development, 1997
This study used event-related potentials (ERP) and visual preference technique to assess 6-month olds' ability to recognize their mothers' face. Results of five experiments suggested that infants can recognize their mothers' face, but the neural processes accompanying recognition depend on the difficulty with which mothers can be discriminated…
Descriptors: Experiments, Familiarity, Infants, Learning Processes

Holliday, Robyn E. – Child Development, 2003
Two experiments examined effects of a cognitive interview on 4- and 8-year-olds' correct recall and subsequent reporting of misinformation. Found that a cognitive interview elicited more correct details than a control interview. Eight-year-olds' reports were more complete than 4-year-olds', with more correct person, action, object, and location…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cross Sectional Studies, Developmentally Appropriate Practices