NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 43 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
LaTourrette, Alexander; Waxman, Sandra R. – Child Development, 2022
Abstract Labeling promotes infants' object categorization even when labels are rare. By 2 years, infants engage in "semi-supervised learning" (SSL), integrating labeled and unlabeled exemplars to learn categories. However, everyday learning contexts pose substantial challenges for infants' SSL. Here, two studies (n = 74, 51% female, 62%…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, Learning Strategies
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stefanie Peykarjou; Stefanie Hoehl; Sabina Pauen – Child Development, 2024
This study investigated the development of rapid visual object categorization. N = 20 adults (Experiment 1), N = 21 five to six-year-old children (Experiment 2), and N = 140 four-, seven-, and eleven-month-old infants (Experiment 3; all predominantly White, 81 females, data collected in 2013-2020) participated in a fast periodic visual stimulation…
Descriptors: Cues, Visual Perception, Child Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Howard, Lauren H.; Henderson, Annette M. E.; Carrazza, Cristina; Woodward, Amanda L. – Child Development, 2015
Although children can use social categories to intelligently select informants, children's preference for in-group informants has not been consistently demonstrated across age and context. This research clarifies the extent to which children use social categories to guide learning by presenting participants with a live or video-recorded…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Children, Imitation, Group Membership
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Casasola, Marianella; Park, Youjeong – Child Development, 2013
Two experiments examined infants' ability to form a spatial category when habituated to few (only 2) or many (6) exemplars of a spatial relation. Sixty-four infants of 10 months and 64 infants of 14 months were habituated to dynamic events in which a toy was placed in a consistent spatial relation ("in" or "on") to a referent…
Descriptors: Infants, Spatial Ability, Classification, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ablow, Jennifer C.; Marks, Amy K.; Shirley Feldman, S.; Huffman, Lynne C. – Child Development, 2013
Associations among 53 primiparous women's Adult Attachment Interview classifications (secure-autonomous vs. insecure-dismissing) and physiological and self-reported responses to infant crying were explored. Heart rate, skin conductance levels, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) were recorded continuously. In response to the cry,…
Descriptors: Correlation, Pregnancy, Measures (Individuals), Security (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yuan, Sylvia; Fisher, Cynthia; Snedeker, Jesse – Child Development, 2012
Two-year-olds use the sentence structures verbs appear in--"subcategorization frames"--to guide verb learning. This is syntactic bootstrapping. This study probed the developmental origins of this ability. The structure-mapping account proposes that children begin with a bias toward one-to-one mapping between nouns in sentences and participant…
Descriptors: Cues, Sentences, Verbs, Nouns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Althaus, Nadja; Mareschal, Denis – Child Development, 2012
This article presents an eye-tracking study using a novel combination of visual saliency maps and "area-of-interest" analyses to explore online feature extraction during category learning in infants. Category learning in 12-month-olds (N = 22) involved a transition from looking at high-saliency image regions to looking at more…
Descriptors: Maps, Classification, Infants, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pruden, Shannon M.; Roseberry, Sarah; Goksun, Tilbe; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
Fundamental to amassing a lexicon of relational terms (i.e., verbs, prepositions) is the ability to abstract and categorize spatial relations such as a figure (e.g., "boy") moving along a path (e.g., "around" the barn). Three studies examine how infants learn to categorize path over changes in "manner," or how an action is performed (e.g., running…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, English, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bornstein, Marc H.; Mash, Clay – Child Development, 2010
What processes do infants employ in categorizing? Infants might categorize on line as they encounter category-related entities; alternatively, infants might depend on prior experience with entities in formulating categories. These alternatives were tested in forty-four 5-month-olds. Infants who were familiarized in the laboratory with a category…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Prior Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ferry, Alissa L.; Hespos, Susan J.; Waxman, Sandra R. – Child Development, 2010
Neonates prefer human speech to other nonlinguistic auditory stimuli. However, it remains an open question whether there are any conceptual consequences of words on object categorization in infants younger than 6 months. The current study examined the influence of words and tones on object categorization in forty-six 3- to 4-month-old infants.…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Neonates, Classification, Speech Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Quinn, Paul C.; Doran, Matthew M.; Reiss, Jason E.; Hoffman, James E. – Child Development, 2009
Previous looking time studies have shown that infants use the heads of cat and dog images to form category representations for these animal classes. The present research used an eye-tracking procedure to determine the time course of attention to the head and whether it reflects a preexisting bias or online learning. Six- to 7-month-olds were…
Descriptors: Attention, Online Courses, Infants, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sherman, Tracy – Child Development, 1985
Infants exposed to a set of artificially-created face stimuli having distinct mean and modal prototypes showed a pattern of behavior predicted by category abstraction models. Infants appeared to abstract, at the time of learning, a feature-count summary of the category displayed. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Ability, Infants, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Younger, Barbara A. – Child Development, 1985
Two experiments investigated infants' use of structural relations in dividing schematic drawings of animals into categories. Results demonstrated subjects' sensitivity to structural information like that thought by Rosch (1978) to exist in the natural world and their ability to segregate items into categories on the basis of clusters of correlated…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Ability, Infants, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sugarman, Susan – Child Development, 1981
The ability of 1- to 3-year-olds to conceptually interrelate objects was studied among eight children each at 12, 18, 24, 30, and 36 months who were given seven free classification tasks containing a scrambled array of eight objects from two classes. Spontaneous manipulations of the subjects were analyzed. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Processes, Infants, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Casasola, Marianella; Bhagwat, Jui – Child Development, 2007
Eighteen-month-olds' spatial categorization was tested when hearing a novel spatial word. Infants formed an abstract categorical representation of support (i.e., placing 1 object on another) when hearing a novel spatial particle during habituation but not when viewing the events in silence. Infants with a productive spatial vocabulary did not…
Descriptors: Nouns, Verbs, Form Classes (Languages), Infants
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3