Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Infants | 25 |
Motion | 25 |
Visual Stimuli | 12 |
Visual Perception | 9 |
Child Development | 7 |
Infant Behavior | 6 |
Attention | 5 |
Perceptual Development | 5 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Comparative Analysis | 4 |
Cognitive Development | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 25 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 24 |
Reports - Research | 23 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 5 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Stahl, Aimee E.; Romberg, Alexa R.; Roseberry, Sarah; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathryn – Child Development, 2014
Throughout their 1st year, infants adeptly detect statistical structure in their environment. However, little is known about whether statistical learning is a primary mechanism for event segmentation. This study directly tests whether statistical learning alone is sufficient to segment continuous events. Twenty-eight 7- to 9-month-old infants…
Descriptors: Infants, Theory of Mind, Cues, Schemata (Cognition)
Frick, Andrea; Wang, Su-hua – Child Development, 2014
Infants' ability to mentally track the orientation of an object during a hidden rotation was investigated (N = 28 in each experiment). A toy on a turntable was fully covered and then rotated 90°. When revealed, the toy had turned with the turntable (probable event), remained at its starting orientation (improbable event in Experiment 1), or…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Kaldy, Zsuzsa; Blaser, Erik – Child Development, 2013
In this study, 6-month-old infants' visual working memory for a static feature (color) and a dynamic feature (rotational motion) was compared. Comparing infants' use of different features can only be done properly if experimental manipulations to those features are equally salient (Kaldy & Blaser, 2009; Kaldy, Blaser, & Leslie,…
Descriptors: Infants, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception, Color
Kahrs, Bjorn A.; Jung, Wendy P.; Lockman, Jeffrey J. – Child Development, 2013
The current study examines the developmental trajectory of banging movements and its implications for tool use development. Twenty (6- to 15-month-old) infants wore reflective markers while banging a handled cube; movements were recorded at 240 Hz. Results indicated that through the second half-year, banging movements undergo developmental changes…
Descriptors: Infants, Motor Development, Psychomotor Skills, Child Development
Zieber, Nicole; Kangas, Ashley; Hock, Alyson; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Child Development, 2014
Adults recognize emotions conveyed by bodies with comparable accuracy to facial emotions. However, no prior study has explored infants' perception of body emotions. In Experiment 1, 6.5-month-olds (n = 32) preferred happy over neutral actions of actors with covered faces in upright but not inverted silent videos. In Experiment 2, infants…
Descriptors: Infants, Emotional Development, Emotional Response, Human Body
Pruden, Shannon M.; Goksun, Tilbe; Roseberry, Sarah; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2012
To learn motion verbs, infants must be sensitive to the specific event features lexicalized in their language. One event feature important for the acquisition of English motion verbs is the manner of motion. This article examines when and how infants detect manners of motion across variations in the figure's path. Experiment 1 shows that 13- to…
Descriptors: Verbs, Infants, Motion, Language Acquisition
Pulverman, Rachel; Song, Lulu; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Pruden, Shannon M.; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
In the world, the manners and paths of motion events take place together, but in language, these features are expressed separately. How do infants learn to process motion events in linguistically appropriate ways? Forty-six English-learning 7- to 9-month-olds were habituated to a motion event in which a character performed both a manner and a…
Descriptors: English, Language Acquisition, Infants, Cognitive Processes
Otsuka, Yumiko; Konishi, Yukuo; Kanazawa, So; Yamaguchi, Masami K.; Abdi, Herve; O'Toole, Alice J. – Child Development, 2009
This study compared 3- to 4-month-olds' recognition of previously unfamiliar faces learned in a moving or a static condition. Infants in the moving condition showed successful recognition with only 30 s familiarization, even when different images of a face were used in the familiarization and test phase (Experiment 1). In contrast, infants in the…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Nonverbal Communication, Visual Stimuli
Yoon, Jennifer M. D.; Johnson, Susan C. – Child Development, 2009
To test the hypothesis that biological motion perception is developmentally integrated with important social cognitive abilities, 12-month-olds (N = 36) were shown a display of a human point-light figure turning to observe a target. Infants spontaneously and reliably followed the figure's "gaze" despite the absence of familiar and socially…
Descriptors: Social Behavior, Motion, Cognitive Ability, Developmental Stages

Kellman, Philip J.; And Others – Child Development, 1986
Results of three experiments support the generalization that early perception of object unity depends on perceived motion, and that the class of effective motions includes, at least, the rigid translations of an object in three-dimensional space. Effective motions may be specified by very different sources of stimulus information. (RH)
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Perception

Van de Walle, Gretchen A.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Child Development, 1996
Investigated 5-month-olds' perception of an object whose center was occluded and whose ends were visible only in succession. Found that infants perceived the object as one connected whole when the ends underwent common motion but not when the ends were stationary. Results suggest that infants perceive object unity but not object form. (Author/BC)
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception

Streri, Arlette; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Child Development, 1989
After haptic habituation to a ring display, infants perceived the rings in two experiments as parts of one connected object. In both haptic and visual modes, infants appeared to perceive object unity by analyzing motion but not by analyzing figural goodness. (RH)
Descriptors: Adults, Infants, Motion, Perception

Byrne, Joseph M.; Horowitz, Frances Degen – Child Development, 1984
Examines discrimination of geometric shapes by three-month-old infants who were presented with geometric stimuli moving laterally at two different velocities. Finds that subjects discriminate between geometric forms at velocities that, according to previous findings, might interfere with shape discrimination. Discusses the possible interactive…
Descriptors: Discrimination Learning, Infants, Motion, Perceptual Development

Nelson, Charles A.; Horowitz, Frances Degen – Child Development, 1983
Holograms of faces were used to study two- and five-month-old infants' discriminations of changes in facial expression and pose when the stimulus was seen to move or to remain stationary. While no evidence was found suggesting that infants preferred the moving face, evidence indicated that motion contrasts facilitate face recognition. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Facial Expressions, Holography, Infants

Nagata, Yoko; Dannemiller, James L. – Child Development, 1996
Assessed 14-week-olds' attention to green or red target objects moving in a field of distracting objects that varied in color. Found that infants' detection of green moving targets was masked in the presence of mixed red and green objects. Masking was not observed for red targets or for green targets in a field of green objects. (BC)
Descriptors: Attention, Color, Infants, Motion
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2