Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Classification | 18 |
Language Acquisition | 18 |
Cognitive Development | 7 |
Infants | 7 |
Preschool Children | 7 |
Language Research | 6 |
Child Language | 5 |
Toddlers | 5 |
Child Development | 4 |
Concept Formation | 4 |
Nouns | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 18 |
Author
Waxman, Sandra R. | 3 |
Gopnik, Alison | 2 |
Smith, Linda B. | 2 |
Bertrand, Jacquelyn | 1 |
Bhagwat, Jui | 1 |
Bianchi, Lydia | 1 |
Casasola, Marianella | 1 |
Chalik, Lisa | 1 |
Davis, Albert J. | 1 |
Dodd, David H. | 1 |
Duke, Nell | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 16 |
Reports - Research | 15 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 2 |
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rhodes, Marjorie; Leslie, Sarah-Jane; Bianchi, Lydia; Chalik, Lisa – Child Development, 2018
Classifying people into categories not only helps humans simplify a complex social world but also contributes to stereotyping and discrimination. This research examines how social categorization develops by testing how language imbues with meaning otherwise arbitrary differences between people. Experimental studies (N = 129) with 2-year-olds…
Descriptors: Classification, Language Role, Stereotypes, Toddlers
Yuan, Sylvia; Fisher, Cynthia; Snedeker, Jesse – Child Development, 2012
Two-year-olds use the sentence structures verbs appear in--"subcategorization frames"--to guide verb learning. This is syntactic bootstrapping. This study probed the developmental origins of this ability. The structure-mapping account proposes that children begin with a bias toward one-to-one mapping between nouns in sentences and participant…
Descriptors: Cues, Sentences, Verbs, Nouns
Pruden, Shannon M.; Roseberry, Sarah; Goksun, Tilbe; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
Fundamental to amassing a lexicon of relational terms (i.e., verbs, prepositions) is the ability to abstract and categorize spatial relations such as a figure (e.g., "boy") moving along a path (e.g., "around" the barn). Three studies examine how infants learn to categorize path over changes in "manner," or how an action is performed (e.g., running…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, English, Language Acquisition
Casasola, Marianella; Bhagwat, Jui – Child Development, 2007
Eighteen-month-olds' spatial categorization was tested when hearing a novel spatial word. Infants formed an abstract categorical representation of support (i.e., placing 1 object on another) when hearing a novel spatial particle during habituation but not when viewing the events in silence. Infants with a productive spatial vocabulary did not…
Descriptors: Nouns, Verbs, Form Classes (Languages), Infants

Gopnik, Alison; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Child Development, 1992
Eighteen-month-old children performed sorting tasks and their parents completed checklists of words used by the children. Children who performed exhaustive grouping, or grouping of objects of different kinds in different locations, were reported as using more words than children who did not perform exhaustive grouping. (BC)
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Language Acquisition, Object Permanence

Waxman, Sandra R.; Hall, D. Geoffrey. – Child Development, 1993
In 2 experiments, 15- and 21-month-old infants were presented with a target object and asked to select an object taxonomically or thematically related to the target object. The target object was introduced with or without a novel nonsense noun. Results indicated that novel nouns focused infants' attention on taxonomic relations. (MDM)
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Language Acquisition, Language Research

Hall, D. Geoffrey; Graham, Susan A. – Child Development, 1999
Three experiments examined role of lexical-form class in preschoolers' establishment of word-to-object mappings in referentially ambiguous situations. Results indicated that preschoolers were most likely to reject two words for the same object if both were proper names, and were less likely to reject if both were adjectives or if one was proper…
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Classification, Form Classes (Languages), Language Acquisition

Gopnik, Alison; Meltzoff, Andrew – Child Development, 1987
Changes in children's categorization behavior between 15 and 21 months of age and the relation of these changes to developments in language, object permanence, and means-end understanding are reported. (PCB)
Descriptors: Behavior Change, Classification, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior

Nelson, Deborah G. Kemler; Russell, Rachel; Duke, Nell; Jones, Kate – Child Development, 2000
Three studies examined lexical categorization in 2-year- olds. Findings indicated that even with minimal opportunities to familiarize themselves with novel artifacts, children generalized their names in accordance with the objects' functions, even when they had to discover the functions on their own or when all the test objects had some…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Generalization

Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa; Thal, Donna J.; Smith, Linda B.; Namy, Laura L. – Child Development, 1997
Three studies examined the developmental relationship between early linguistic and cognitive achievements. Findings showed that children's ability to classify objects in a spatial or temporal order was independent of advances in productive vocabulary growth, suggesting that developments in categorization and naming depend on abilities in addition…
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Classification, Individual Development

Liu, Jing; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Sak, Kimberly – Child Development, 2001
Six match-to-sample picture/object selection experiments explored 3- to 5-year-olds' knowledge about superordinate words and acquisition of this knowledge. Findings indicated that number of standards (one versus two), types of standards (different versus same basic-level categories), and nature of representation (pictures versus objects)…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Cues

Neimark, Edith D. – Child Development, 1974
Subjects in grades 2, 6, and college were asked to sort 50 pictures according to several class labels, each with a functional equivalent. (ST)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development

Smith, Linda B.; And Others – Child Development, 1986
Examines how reference points for the categorical interpretation of high and low (adjectives) were defined by three- to five-year-old children and adults. Shows categorical interpretations of relative terms to be complex dependent. (HOD)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Adults, Classification, Cognitive Ability

Tomikawa, Sandra A.; Dodd, David H. – Child Development, 1980
In a series of five experiments, young children (two- and three-year-olds) were presented with novel objects in which perceptual and functional features varied independently. Results indicate that early conceptualizations and word meanings are perceptually based when perceptual and functional features are independently available. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Child Language, Classification, Concept Formation, Criteria

Klibanoff, Raquel S.; Waxman, Sandra R. – Child Development, 2000
Examined preschoolers' ability to map novel adjectives to object properties in two experiments. Found that 4-year-olds could extend novel adjectives from target to matching test object whether objects were drawn from same, or different, basic level categories. If 3-year-olds' first extended a novel adjective to objects in the same basic level…
Descriptors: Adjectives, Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2