Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Language Processing | 6 |
Phonology | 6 |
Language Acquisition | 5 |
Child Development | 3 |
Word Recognition | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
Language Skills | 2 |
Pictorial Stimuli | 2 |
Task Analysis | 2 |
Acoustics | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Alan Langus | 1 |
Amanda Saksida | 1 |
Beckman, Mary E. | 1 |
Bergelson, Elika | 1 |
Edwards, Jan | 1 |
Mani, Nivedita | 1 |
McBride-Chang, Catherine | 1 |
Pennington, Bruce F. | 1 |
Peperkamp, Sharon | 1 |
Skoruppa, Katrin | 1 |
Swingley, Daniel | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 5 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
France | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Amanda Saksida; Alan Langus – Child Development, 2024
The account that word learning starts in earnest during the second year of life, when infants have mastered the disambiguation skills, has recently been challenged by evidence that infants during the first year already know many common words. The preliminary ability to rapidly map and disambiguate linguistic labels was tested in Italian-speaking…
Descriptors: Naming, Infants, Cognitive Mapping, Vocabulary Development
Bergelson, Elika; Swingley, Daniel – Child Development, 2018
To understand spoken words, listeners must appropriately interpret co-occurring talker characteristics and speech sound content. This ability was tested in 6- to 14-months-olds by measuring their looking to named food and body part images. In the "new talker" condition (n = 90), pictures were named by an unfamiliar voice; in the…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Processing, Infant Behavior, Food
Skoruppa, Katrin; Mani, Nivedita; Peperkamp, Sharon – Child Development, 2013
Using a picture pointing task, this study examines toddlers' processing of phonological alternations that trigger sound changes in connected speech. Three experiments investigate whether 2;5- to 3-year-old children take into account assimilations--processes by which phonological features of one sound spread to adjacent sounds--for the purpose of…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Toddlers, Language Processing, Phonology

McBride-Chang, Catherine – Child Development, 1996
Examined the associations among speech perception, phonological awareness, naming speed, verbal memory, and word reading. Multiple measures were administered to 136 3rd- and 4th-grade children. Results indicated that naming speed was particularly highly associated with speech perception, whereas phonological awareness was substantially correlated…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages, Language Acquisition, Language Processing

Pennington, Bruce F.; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Four experiments with familial and clinical dyslexics aimed at defining primary processing deficits in adult dyslexia. Processes studied included phoneme perception and awareness, lexical retrieval, articulatory speed, and short-term memory. Only phoneme awareness met the criteria for primary deficit. Clinical dyslexics exhibited short-term memory…
Descriptors: Adults, Articulation (Speech), Dyslexia, Encoding (Psychology)

Beckman, Mary E.; Edwards, Jan – Child Development, 2000
Presents evidence from studies on adults' language processing and children's language acquisition that the lexicon is at the core of grammatical generalizations at several levels of representation. Proposes that phonological acquisition might provide the bootstrapping into grammatical generalization in general. Concludes that age-appropriate…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Development, Children