Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Grade 3 | 6 |
Problem Solving | 6 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Conceptual Tempo | 2 |
Elementary School Students | 2 |
Grade 1 | 2 |
Grade 2 | 2 |
Grade 5 | 2 |
Individual Differences | 2 |
Intervention | 2 |
Learning Processes | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 6 |
Author
Abaied, Jamie L. | 1 |
Agoston, Anna Monica | 1 |
Amsterlaw, Jennifer | 1 |
Flynn, Megan | 1 |
Mann, Leon | 1 |
McNeil, Nicole M. | 1 |
Offenbach, Stuart I. | 1 |
Rudolph, Karen D. | 1 |
Sugimura, Niwako | 1 |
Zelniker, Tamar | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 4 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Education Level
Grade 3 | 3 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Grade 2 | 2 |
Grade 5 | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Matching Familiar Figures Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Rudolph, Karen D.; Abaied, Jamie L.; Flynn, Megan; Sugimura, Niwako; Agoston, Anna Monica – Child Development, 2011
This research explored the contribution of social goal orientation, specifically, development (improving social skills and relationships), demonstration-approach (gaining positive judgments), and demonstration-avoidance (minimizing negative judgments). Children (N = 373; M age = 7.97, SD = 0.34) were followed from 2nd to 3rd grades. Validity of…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Goal Orientation, Problem Solving, Social Adjustment
McNeil, Nicole M. – Child Development, 2008
Do typical arithmetic problems hinder learning of mathematical equivalence? Second and third graders (7-9 years old; N= 80) received lessons on mathematical equivalence either with or without typical arithmetic problems (e.g., 15 + 13 = 28 vs. 28 = 28, respectively). Children then solved math equivalence problems (e.g., 3 + 9 + 5 = 6 + __),…
Descriptors: Children, Grade 2, Grade 3, Grade 5

Mann, Leon – Child Development, 1973
It was concluded that in grade school children the tendency to be reflective in problem solving also entails a disposition to take longer in making decisions. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Conceptual Tempo, Decision Making, Grade 1
Amsterlaw, Jennifer – Child Development, 2006
Two studies investigated children's metacognition about everyday reasoning, assessing how they distinguish reasoning from nonreasoning and "good" reasoning from "bad." In Study 1, 80 1st graders (6-7 years), 3rd graders (8-9 years), 5th graders (10-11 years), and adults (18+ years) evaluated scenarios where people (a) used reasoning, (b) solved…
Descriptors: Grade 1, Grade 5, Grade 3, Metacognition

Zelniker, Tamar; And Others – Child Development, 1972
Descriptors: Attention Span, Conceptual Tempo, Data Analysis, Eye Fixations

Offenbach, Stuart I. – Child Development, 1980
According to Hypothesis (H) theory, learning should be very difficult when the number of Hs the subject samples from is very large and/or the correct H is not available. These assumptions were tested with third- and fourth-grade children. In general, results supported these assumptions. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Discrimination Learning, Elementary School Students, Failure