NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Davidov, Maayan; Vaish, Amrisha; Knafo-Noam, Ariel; Hastings, Paul D. – Child Development, 2016
Prosocial behavior is versatile, multifaceted, and complex. This special section seeks to advance coherent, integrative understanding of prosocial development by addressing this topic through the prism of motivations. This conceptual Introduction presents key ideas that provide a framework for thinking about motivation for prosocial behavior and…
Descriptors: Prosocial Behavior, Motivation, Developmental Stages, Environmental Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Umana-Taylor, Adriana J.; Gonzales-Backen, Melinda A.; Guimond, Amy B. – Child Development, 2009
The current longitudinal study of 323 Latino adolescents (50.5% male; M age = 15.31 years) examined whether ethnic identity exploration, resolution, and affirmation demonstrated significant growth over a 4-year period and whether growth in ethnic identity predicted growth in self-esteem. Findings from multiple-group latent growth curve models…
Descriptors: Ethnicity, Females, Late Adolescents, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Degnan, Kathryn A.; Calkins, Susan D.; Keane, Susan P.; Hill-Soderlund, Ashley L. – Child Development, 2008
Disruptive behavior, including aggression, defiance, and temper tantrums, typically peaks in early toddlerhood and decreases by school entry; however, some children do not show this normative decline. The current study examined disruptive behavior in 318 boys and girls at 2, 4, and 5 years of age and frustration reactivity, physiological…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Psychopathology, Profiles, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sayfan, Liat; Lagattuta, Kristin Hansen – Child Development, 2008
Three-, 5-, and 7-year-olds and adults (N = 64) listened to stories depicting 2 protagonists of different ages (infant and child or child and grownup) that encounter an entity that looks like a real (e.g., a snake) or an imaginary (e.g., a ghost) fear-inducing creature. Participants predicted and explained each protagonist's intensity of fear.…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Infants, Fear, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Benner, Aprile D.; Graham, Sandra – Child Development, 2009
The high school transition was examined in an ethnically diverse, urban sample of 1,979 adolescents, followed from 7th to 10th grade (M[subscript age] = 14.6, SD = 0.37 in 7th grade). Twice annually, data were gathered on adolescents' perceptions of school climate, psychological functioning, and academic behaviors. Piecewise growth modeling…
Descriptors: High Schools, Middle Schools, Racial Differences, African American Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lockhart, Kristi L.; Chang, Bernard; Story, Tyler – Child Development, 2002
Four studies explored children's beliefs about the stability of positive traits among three groups. Findings indicated that younger children were more likely than older children or adults to believe that negative physical and psychological traits would change positively, that they could control the expression of a trait, and that extreme positive…
Descriptors: Adults, Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lagattuta, Kristin Hansen – Child Development, 2005
This research investigated 4- through 7-year-olds' and adults' (n=64) concepts about the emotional consequences of desire fulfillment versus desire inhibition in situations where people's desires conflict with prohibitive rules. Results revealed developmental increases in attributing positive or mixed emotions to story characters that make…
Descriptors: Emotional Response, Age Differences, Young Children, Adults