Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Semantics | 7 |
Pictorial Stimuli | 4 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Elementary School Students | 3 |
Verbal Stimuli | 3 |
College Students | 2 |
Interference (Language) | 2 |
Learning Modalities | 2 |
Toddlers | 2 |
Visual Stimuli | 2 |
Word Recognition | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 7 |
Author
Rosinski, Richard R. | 2 |
Baron-Cohen, Simon | 1 |
Duta, Mihaela | 1 |
Ehri, Linnea C. | 1 |
Golinkoff, Roberta M. | 1 |
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy | 1 |
Pezdek, Kathy | 1 |
Plunkett, Kim | 1 |
Pruden, Shannon M. | 1 |
Pulverman, Rachel | 1 |
Song, Lulu | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 4 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Duta, Mihaela; Plunkett, Kim – Child Development, 2023
We present a neural network model of referent identification in a visual world task. Inputs are visual representations of item pairs unfolding with sequences of phonemes identifying the target item. The model is trained to output the semantic representation of the target and to suppress the distractor. The training set uses a 200-word lexicon…
Descriptors: Networks, Models, Brain, Child Language
Pulverman, Rachel; Song, Lulu; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Pruden, Shannon M.; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
In the world, the manners and paths of motion events take place together, but in language, these features are expressed separately. How do infants learn to process motion events in linguistically appropriate ways? Forty-six English-learning 7- to 9-month-olds were habituated to a motion event in which a character performed both a manner and a…
Descriptors: English, Language Acquisition, Infants, Cognitive Processes

Rosinski, Richard R. – Child Development, 1977
Analysis of the performance of second-, fourth-, sixth-grade, and college-level subjects on picture-word interference tasks indicated that distractor words belonging to the same semantic category as pictures produced more interference than either unrelated words or nonsense trigrams. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: College Students, Elementary School Students, Interference (Language), Learning Modalities

Pezdek, Kathy – Child Development, 1980
Examines life-span developmental differences in spontaneous integration of semantically relevant material presented in pictures and sentences. Elementary school students, high school students, and adults were tested. (Author/SS)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Comprehension, Elementary School Students

Ehri, Linnea C. – Child Development, 1977
Third- and sixth-grade readers were asked to label sets of pictures printed with distracting words (either nouns, adjectives, or functors) and nonsense syllables. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Elementary Education, Function Words, Interference (Language)

Rosinski, Richard R.; And Others – Child Development, 1975
Presents two experiments which measured latencies in a picture-word interference task to assess semantic processing. Results suggest that picture-word interference is partly semantically based and that children and adults experience an equivalent amount of semantic interference. (Author/SDH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, College Students, Elementary School Students, Pictorial Stimuli

Baron-Cohen, Simon; And Others – Child Development, 1997
Two studies of toddlers and children with autism, mentally handicapped children, and normal toddlers examined whether autistic toddlers used Speaker's Direction of Gaze (SDG) strategy or less powerful Listener's Direction of Gaze (LDG) strategy to learn a word for a novel object. Results suggest autistic toddlers are insensitive to speaker's gaze…
Descriptors: Autism, Cognitive Processes, Context Clues, Language Processing