Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 44 |
Social Cognition | 44 |
Children | 18 |
Age Differences | 13 |
Child Development | 8 |
Preschool Children | 8 |
Young Children | 8 |
Adolescents | 7 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Adults | 5 |
Concept Formation | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 44 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 44 |
Reports - Research | 39 |
Information Analyses | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 5 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Tenenbaum, Harriet R.; Ruck, Martin D. – Child Development, 2012
This study examined British young people's understanding of the rights of asylum-seeking young people. Two hundred sixty participants (11-24 years) were read vignettes involving asylum-seeking young people's religious and nonreligious self-determination and nurturance rights. Religious rights were more likely to be endorsed than nonreligious…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Young Adults, Adolescents, Foreign Countries
Svetlova, Margarita; Nichols, Sara R.; Brownell, Celia A. – Child Development, 2010
The study explored how the meaning of prosocial behavior changes over toddlerhood. Sixty-five 18- and 30-month-olds could help an adult in 3 contexts: instrumental (action based), empathic (emotion based), and altruistic (costly). Children at both ages helped readily in instrumental tasks. For 18-month-olds, empathic helping was significantly more…
Descriptors: Cues, Prosocial Behavior, Altruism, Toddlers
Filippova, Eva; Astington, Janet Wilde – Child Development, 2008
This study describes the development of social reasoning in school-age children. An irony task is used to assess 5-, 7-, and 9-year-olds' (N = 72) and adults' (N = 24) recursive understanding of others' minds. Guttman scale analysis demonstrates that in order to understand a speaker's communicative intention, a child needs to recognize the…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Language Aptitude, Cognitive Development, Social Cognition
Hoehl, Stefanie; Reid, Vincent M.; Parise, Eugenio; Handl, Andrea; Palumbo, Letizia; Striano, Tricia – Child Development, 2009
The importance of eye gaze as a means of communication is indisputable. However, there is debate about whether there is a dedicated neural module, which functions as an eye gaze detector and when infants are able to use eye gaze cues in a referential way. The application of neuroscience methodologies to developmental psychology has provided new…
Descriptors: Child Development, Infants, Cues, Eye Movements
Yoon, Jennifer M. D.; Johnson, Susan C. – Child Development, 2009
To test the hypothesis that biological motion perception is developmentally integrated with important social cognitive abilities, 12-month-olds (N = 36) were shown a display of a human point-light figure turning to observe a target. Infants spontaneously and reliably followed the figure's "gaze" despite the absence of familiar and socially…
Descriptors: Social Behavior, Motion, Cognitive Ability, Developmental Stages
Corina, David; Singleton, Jenny – Child Development, 2009
The condition of deafness presents a developmental context that provides insight into the biological, cultural, and linguistic factors underlying the development of neural systems that impact social cognition. Studies of visual attention, behavioral regulation, language development, and face and human action perception are discussed. Visually…
Descriptors: Social Environment, Linguistics, Deafness, Caregivers
Nicolopoulou, Ageliki; Richner, Elizabeth S. – Child Development, 2007
This study addressed a puzzling discrepancy in existing research about when children achieve and manifest a mentalistic conception of the person. Narrative research suggests that children do not represent characters as mental agents until middle childhood, whereas social cognition research places this understanding at around 4 years. Using a…
Descriptors: Social Cognition, Preschool Curriculum, Cognitive Development, Preschool Children

Moses, Louis J.; Baldwin, Dare A.; Rosicky, Julie G.; Tidball, Glynnis – Child Development, 2001
Examined in two studies referential understanding in 12- and 18-month-olds' responses to another's emotional outburst. Found that infants relied on the presence versus absence of referential cues to determine whether an emotional message should be linked with a salient object and they actively consulted referential cues to disambiguate the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cues, Emotional Development, Infants

Barrett, Justin L.; Richert, Rebekah A.; Driesenga, Amanda – Child Development, 2001
Three experiments examined assumption that children attribute human properties to nonhuman agents. Two- to 8-year-olds participated in false-belief tests concerning human and various nonhuman agents, including animals and God, and in a modified perspective-taking task including nonhuman agents. Results suggested that children do not consistently…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Childhood Attitudes, Children, Cognitive Development

Shantz, Carolyn Uhlinger – Child Development, 1987
Interpersonal conflicts of children in natural settings are the focus of this review of the empirical literature in social and cognitive development and sociolinguistics. The central role of conflict in various developmental theories is outlined. Studies indicate substantial relations between children's socio-cognitive functioning and their…
Descriptors: Aggression, Children, Cognitive Development, Conflict

Tunmer, William E. – Child Development, 1985
Acquisition of sentient-nonsentient distinction in 48 children between four- and seven-years-of-age occurred later than animate-inanimate distinction. The children's use of naturalistic or nonnaturalistic explanations depended on the logical nature of events in which objects were involved rather than familiarity with objects themselves. Ability to…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation, Foreign Countries

Schult, Carolyn A. – Child Development, 2002
To assess children's understanding of intentions as distinct from desires, this study presented 3- to 7-year-olds and adults with situations in which intentions were satisfied but desires were not, or vice versa, in a story-comprehension task and target-hitting game. Findings indicated that younger children were unable to differentiate desires and…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development

Chandler, Michael; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Investigated the ability of 56 children of 2-4 years to deceptively lead others into false beliefs. Results show that 2 1/2-year-olds already practice a variety of deceptive strategies that presuppose an operative theory of mind. (RJC)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Deception

Moore, Chris; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Two studies showed that children of about four years of age had begun to understand that beliefs may be held with differing degrees of certainty. This understanding was tied to the children's understanding of the nature of beliefs and the distinction between appearance and reality. (PCB)
Descriptors: Beliefs, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Foreign Countries

Jordan, Valerie Barnes – Child Development, 1980
Piaget's conservation paradigm was used to assess five- to seven-year-old children's understanding of the permanence of various kinship roles. Children's conservation was studied by applying certain transformations on single- and multiple-kinship role combinations. Kinship conservation developed gradually in this age range. Females' performance…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Conservation (Concept), Sex Differences