NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 15 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Banerjee, Robin; Bennett, Mark; Luke, Nikki – Child Development, 2012
Rule violations are likely to serve as key contexts for learning to reason about public identity. In an initial study with 91 children aged 4-9 years, social emotions and self-presentational concerns were more likely to be cited when children were responding to hypothetical vignettes involving social-conventional rather than moral violations. In 2…
Descriptors: Vignettes, Video Technology, Social Behavior, Behavior Standards
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Killen, Melanie; Mulvey, Kelly Lynn; Hitti, Aline – Child Development, 2013
"Interpersonal" rejection and "intergroup" exclusion in childhood reflect different, but complementary, aspects of child development. Interpersonal rejection focuses on individual differences in personality traits, such as wariness and being fearful, to explain bully-victim relationships. In contrast, intergroup exclusion focuses on how in-group…
Descriptors: Rejection (Psychology), Social Isolation, Child Development, Interpersonal Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Flavell, John H.; And Others – Child Development, 1992
Children between three and five years of age were told or shown that story characters held beliefs that differed from their own beliefs concerning physical facts, moral values, social conventions, personal values, and ownership. Found that three year olds had difficulty attributing to others beliefs that differed from their own. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Beliefs, Moral Values, Ownership
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Olthof, Tjeert; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Investigated the potential cognitive basis for anger in 80 children of 5, 6, 9, 11, and 15 years. Results showed that avoidability, intentionality, and motive acceptability similarly affected children's anger-related and moral judgments. Use of personal responsibility dimensions was associated with lower anger judgments. (RJC)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Anger, Child Responsibility, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nunner-Winkler, Gertrud; Sodian, Beate – Child Development, 1988
Studied the emotional attributions which 60 children aged four-eight gave to a story figure who violated a moral rule. Results suggested a clear change from outcome-oriented toward morally oriented attributions to a moral wrongdoer between the different age groups. (RJC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Empathy, Inferences, Moral Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Shweder, Richard A. – Child Development, 1990
The moral realism of everyday life is neither Piaget's childlike egocentrism nor Gabennesch's reification. Natural moral law is seen by Turiel, a cognitivist, as a code of harm, rights, and justice. Other cognitivists accept codes of duty and natural order. (BC)
Descriptors: Behavior Standards, Justice, Moral Development, Moral Values
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Helwig, Charles C.; And Others – Child Development, 1990
Moral judgments are an important aspect of social reasoning, not arbitrary products of social formations. Maintains that Gabennesch relegates moral concepts to reification, failing to account for the distinctions between conventionality and moral concepts. (BC)
Descriptors: Children, Ethics, Ethnocentrism, Moral Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nicholls, John G.; Thorkildsen, Theresa A. – Child Development, 1988
First-, third-, and fifth-grade students saw matters involving intellectual conventions and personal preference as more variable across time and space than matters involving logic and physical laws. Furthermore, intellectual conventions were seen as legitimately changeable by social consensus and school authorities, but not on the basis of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Structures, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Evaluative Thinking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smetana, Judith G.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Examined three and four year olds' judgments about transgressions. Children judged moral transgressions to be more serious, punishable, and wrong than conventional transgressions; hypothetical transgressions to be more wrong independent of rules than actual transgressions; and hypothetical moral transgressions to be more independent of rules than…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Day Care Centers, Moral Values
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ferguson, Tamara J.; Rule, Brendan Gail – Child Development, 1988
Examines the effect of the severity and causal nature of initial provocation on seventy-two five- to ten-year-old children's retaliation judgments. Children's perceptions of the provocation were more differentiated than those that had previously been reported. Evaluation of the retaliator varied systematically according to perceptions of the…
Descriptors: Aggression, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Evaluative Thinking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McGillicuddy-De Lisi, Ann V.; And Others – Child Development, 1994
Investigated how children's decisions about allocating money to story characters were affected by the relationship (friends versus strangers) among the characters. Children's rationales for their decisions showed that equality was the most salient principle for decisions at all ages and that older children provided rationales based on benevolence…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Age Differences, Child Development, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gabennesch, Howard – Child Development, 1990
Some studies indicate that individuals recognize conventional norms as social contrivances; others, that individuals reify social formations as something other than social products. Questions about comparatively transparent rules and the use of simplistic questions for complex phenomena give an exaggerated portrayal of individuals' awareness of…
Descriptors: Adults, Behavior Standards, Children, Ethnocentrism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rotheram-Borus, Mary Jane; Phinney, Jean S. – Child Development, 1990
In two studies, a total of 213 Black and Mexican-American elementary school children were asked to respond to 8 videotaped scenes of everyday social encounters that occur at school. Actors were unfamiliar peers from the same ethnic group as the subjects. (PCB)
Descriptors: Acculturation, Age Differences, Blacks, Cultural Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Killen, Melanie; Stangor, Charles – Child Development, 2001
Investigated age and context differences in children's judgments about excluding peers from group activities on the basis of gender and race. Found that the vast majority of children rejected exclusion in contexts in which only stereotypes justified exclusion. Older children (13 years) were more likely to allow exclusion than younger (7 and 10…
Descriptors: Adolescent Attitudes, Age Differences, Childhood Attitudes, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Helwig, Charles C.; Arnold, Mary Louise; Tan, Dingliang; Boyd, Dwight – Child Development, 2003
This study explored judgments and reasoning of Chinese 13- to 18-year-olds regarding making decisions involving children in peer, family, and school contexts. Findings indicated that judgments and reasoning about decision-making varied by social context and by the decision under consideration. Evaluations of procedures became more differentiated…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Adult Child Relationship, Age Differences, Childrens Rights