NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 15 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frick, Andrea; Wang, Su-hua – Child Development, 2014
Infants' ability to mentally track the orientation of an object during a hidden rotation was investigated (N = 28 in each experiment). A toy on a turntable was fully covered and then rotated 90°. When revealed, the toy had turned with the turntable (probable event), remained at its starting orientation (improbable event in Experiment 1), or…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Negen, James; Sarnecka, Barbara W. – Child Development, 2012
How is number-concept acquisition related to overall language development? Experiments 1 and 2 measured number-word knowledge and general vocabulary in a total of 59 children, ages 30-60 months. A strong correlation was found between number-word knowledge and vocabulary, independent of the child's age, contrary to previous results (D. Ansari et…
Descriptors: Young Children, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Development, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bremner, J. Gavin; Slater, Alan M.; Johnson, Scott P.; Mason, Uschi C.; Spring, Jo; Bremner, Maggie E. – Child Development, 2011
From birth, infants detect associations between the locations of static visual objects and sounds they emit, but there is limited evidence regarding their sensitivity to the dynamic equivalent when a sound-emitting object moves. In 4 experiments involving thirty-six 2-month-olds, forty-eight 5-month-olds, and forty-eight 8-month-olds, we…
Descriptors: Infants, Habituation, Early Childhood Education, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Van de Walle, Gretchen A.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Child Development, 1996
Investigated 5-month-olds' perception of an object whose center was occluded and whose ends were visible only in succession. Found that infants perceived the object as one connected whole when the ends underwent common motion but not when the ends were stationary. Results suggest that infants perceive object unity but not object form. (Author/BC)
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Granrud, Carl E.; And Others – Child Development, 1984
A total of 20 infants either five or seven months of age viewed computer-generated random-lot displays in which accretion and deletion of texture provided the only information for contours. Infants of both age groups showed significant preferences to reach for the apparently nearer regions in the displays. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infants, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Liben, Lynn S.; Yekel, Candice A. – Child Development, 1996
Preschoolers placed stickers on maps to show locations of objects currently in view. Vantage point (eye-level versus raised), map form (plan versus oblique), and item type (floor versus furniture location) were varied. Results showed that using an oblique map first aided subsequent performance on a plan map. Subjects performed worse on floor…
Descriptors: Map Skills, Preschool Children, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Longo, Matthew R.; Kenny, Sarah – Child Development, 2007
The perceived spatiotemporal continuity of objects depends on the way they appear and disappear as they move in the spatial layout. This study investigated whether infants' predictive tracking of a briefly occluded object is sensitive to the manner by which the object disappears and reappears. Five-, 7-, and 9-month-old infants were shown a ball…
Descriptors: Kinetics, Infants, Visual Perception, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goswami, Usha – Child Development, 1995
In three experiments, three- and four-year olds were asked to map relative size from one array of objects to another, map relative size to relative proportion, and map relative size to a variety of perceptual dimensions. Children were able to make relational mappings based on size when spatial positions and concrete representations of size of…
Descriptors: Analogy, Perceptual Development, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pillow, Bradford H.; Flavell, John H. – Child Development, 1986
Four experiments investigated three- and four-year-old children's knowledge of projective size-distance and projective shape-orientation relationships. Results indicated that preschool children's understanding of these relationships seems at least partly cognitive rather than wholly perceptive, providing further evidence for the acquisition of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Concept Formation, Preschool Children, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lew, Adina R.; Bremner, J. Gavin; Lefkovitch, Leonard P. – Child Development, 2000
Examined development of infants' relational coding in spatial orientation problems. Found that 6-month-olds performed poorly in a peekaboo task in which they had to turn to a target after displacement to a novel position and direction. Twelve- month-olds solved the tasks whether or not target was located between two landmarks; 8.5-month-olds…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comparative Analysis, Infants, Performance Factors
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fisher, Celia B. – Child Development, 1979
In Experiment I, 24 preschoolers were tested on left-right, vertical-horizontal, and mirror-image oblique discriminations under essentially context-free conditions. Experiment II contrasted children's performance under context-free conditions with their ability to discriminate orientation in the presence of external visual cues. (RH)
Descriptors: Cues, Memory, Orientation, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McKenzie, B. E.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Two experiments found that (1) by age 8 months infants perceived that leaning extends their effective reaching space to grasp objects; (2) by 10 months they perceived the effective limits of leaning and reaching; and (3) by 12 months they began to perceive how this space may be extended by a mechanical aid. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Infants, Perceptual Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Stiles, Joan; And Others – Child Development, 1991
In two experiments, preschool children and adults were asked to judge which way an equilateral triangle was pointing under several contextual conditions. Results indicated that children and adults attended to both global and local levels of a pattern. (BC)
Descriptors: Context Effect, Geometric Constructions, Preschool Children, Preschool Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fabricius, William V.; Wellman, Henry M. – Child Development, 1993
Investigated 4-6 year olds' ability to compare the distances covered by a direct and an indirect route to a location. Although many children believed that both routes covered the same distance, about 40% of the four year olds could explain why the direct route was shorter. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Distance, Early Childhood Education, Perceptual Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sandberg, Elisabeth Hollister; And Others – Child Development, 1996
Two studies of development of spatial representation with two dimensions found that children as young as five years use the same two independent dimensions in fine-grained spatial coding of location in a circle as adults use--radius and angle. The adult pattern, where angle as well as radius is coded hierarchically, emerges by nine years. (HTH)
Descriptors: Adults, Classification, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation