Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Classification | 10 |
Task Analysis | 10 |
Child Development | 5 |
Preschool Children | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 3 |
Visual Stimuli | 3 |
Animals | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Concept Formation | 2 |
Developmental Stages | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 10 |
Author
Ash, Michael J. | 1 |
Caruso, John L. | 1 |
Dick, Anthony Steven | 1 |
Dweck, Carol S. | 1 |
Gela, Katherine | 1 |
Goksun, Tilbe | 1 |
Golinkoff, Roberta M. | 1 |
Hayes, Brett K. | 1 |
Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy | 1 |
Margett, Tessa E. | 1 |
Markman, Ellen M. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 7 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Stefanie Peykarjou; Stefanie Hoehl; Sabina Pauen – Child Development, 2024
This study investigated the development of rapid visual object categorization. N = 20 adults (Experiment 1), N = 21 five to six-year-old children (Experiment 2), and N = 140 four-, seven-, and eleven-month-old infants (Experiment 3; all predominantly White, 81 females, data collected in 2013-2020) participated in a fast periodic visual stimulation…
Descriptors: Cues, Visual Perception, Child Development, Infants
Wetzel, Nicole; Scharf, Florian; Widmann, Andreas – Child Development, 2019
Attention control abilities are relevant for learning success. Little is known about the development of audio-visual attention in early childhood. Four groups of children between the ages of 4 and 10 years and adults performed an audio-visual distraction paradigm (N = 106). Multilevel analyses revealed increased reaction times in a visual…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Auditory Stimuli, Visual Stimuli, Task Analysis
Master, Allison; Markman, Ellen M.; Dweck, Carol S. – Child Development, 2012
Can young children, forming expectations about the social world, capture differences among people without falling into the pitfalls of categorization? Categorization often leads to exaggerating differences between groups and minimizing differences within groups, resulting in stereotyping. Six studies with 4-year-old children (N = 214)…
Descriptors: Classification, Inferences, Social Attitudes, Child Development
Pruden, Shannon M.; Roseberry, Sarah; Goksun, Tilbe; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2013
Fundamental to amassing a lexicon of relational terms (i.e., verbs, prepositions) is the ability to abstract and categorize spatial relations such as a figure (e.g., "boy") moving along a path (e.g., "around" the barn). Three studies examine how infants learn to categorize path over changes in "manner," or how an action is performed (e.g., running…
Descriptors: Infants, Classification, English, Language Acquisition
Margett, Tessa E.; Witherington, David C. – Child Development, 2011
This study investigated preschoolers' living kinds conceptualization by employing an extensive stimulus set and alternate indices of understanding. Thirty-four 3- to 5-year-olds and 36 adult undergraduates completed 3 testing phases involving 4 object classes: plants, animals, mobile, and immobile artifacts. The phases involved inquiries…
Descriptors: Testing, Preschool Children, Undergraduate Students, Biology
Hayes, Brett K.; Younger, Katherine – Child Development, 2004
Three experiments examined the changes in category representation that take place when children use exemplars for tasks other than classification. In Experiments 1 and 2, 6- and 10-year-old children learned to classify exemplars of a novel category and then used the same exemplars in an inferential prediction task. In a subsequent classification…
Descriptors: Classification, Task Analysis, Children, Inferences
Muller, Ulrich; Dick, Anthony Steven; Gela, Katherine; Overton, Willis F.; Zelazo, Philip David – Child Development, 2006
Four experiments examined the development of negative priming (NP) in 3-5-year-old children using as a measure of children's executive function (EF) the dimensional change card sort (DCCS) task. In the NP version of the DCCS, the values of the sorting dimension that is relevant during the preswitch phase are removed during the postswitch phase.…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Classification, Task Analysis, Measures (Individuals)

Thompson, Spencer K. – Child Development, 1975
A series of tests was designed for 24-, 30-, and 36-month-olds to measure their ability to apply various gender labels to the appropriate sexes, their capacity to place themselves in their own gender category, and their usage of labels to guide preference behavior. (Author/CS)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Identification (Psychology), Preschool Children

Ash, Michael J. – Child Development, 1975
In an attempt to examine the generalizability of the Kendler S-R mediational model of reversal-shift behavior, 60 third-grade children were classified as either verbal mediators or nonmediators on the basis of their performance on an optional-shift discrimination problem. The children's performances were then evaluated on three tasks. (Author/CS)
Descriptors: Classification, Cluster Grouping, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students

Caruso, John L.; Resnick, Lauren B. – Child Development, 1972
The present study is based on a form of task analysis that explicates in detail the hypothesized solution behavior of skilled performers of the task. (Authors)
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Data Analysis, Kindergarten Children