NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 34 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cimpian, Andrei; Scott, Rose M. – Cognition, 2012
The ability to acquire and store generic information (that is, information about entire categories) is at the core of human cognition. Remarkably, even young children place special value on generic information, often inferring that it holds important insights about the world. Here, we tested whether children's assumptions about the nature of…
Descriptors: Social Cognition, Language Acquisition, Experiments, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rottman, Joshua; Kelemen, Deborah – Cognition, 2012
The traditional cognitive developmental perspective on moral acquisition posits that children actively construct moral beliefs by assessing the negative impacts of antisocial behaviors. This account is not easily applied to actions that are considered immoral despite lacking consequences for others' welfare. We studied the moralization of…
Descriptors: Norms, Moral Values, Moral Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rowland, Caroline F.; Chang, Franklin; Ambridge, Ben; Pine, Julian M.; Lieven, Elena V. M. – Cognition, 2012
Structural priming paradigms have been influential in shaping theories of adult sentence processing and theories of syntactic development. However, until recently there have been few attempts to provide an integrated account that explains both adult and developmental data. The aim of the present paper was to begin the process of integration by…
Descriptors: Priming, Evidence, Sentences, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roder, Brigitte; Pagel, Birthe; Heed, Tobias – Cognition, 2013
The integrated use of spatial and temporal information seems to support the separation of two sensory streams. The present study tested whether this facilitation depends on the encoding of sensory stimuli in externally anchored spatial coordinate systems. Fifty-nine children between 5 and 12 years as well as 12 young adults performed a crossmodal…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes, Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Berteletti, Ilaria; Lucangeli, Daniela; Zorzi, Marco – Cognition, 2012
The representation of numerical and non-numerical ordered sequences was investigated in children from preschool to grade 3. The child's conception of how sequence items map onto a spatial scale was tested using the Number-to-Position task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and new variants of the task designed to probe the representation of the alphabet…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Investigations, Preschool Education, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schapiro, Anna C.; McClelland, James L. – Cognition, 2009
A connectionist model of the balance scale task is presented which exhibits developmental transitions between "Rule I" and "Rule II" behavior [Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. "Cognitive Psychology," 8, 481-520.] as well as the "catastrophe flags" seen in data from Jansen and van der Maas [Jansen, B. R. J., & van der…
Descriptors: Cognitive Psychology, Cognitive Development, Children, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morrison, Catriona M.; Conway, Martin A. – Cognition, 2010
In two experiments autobiographical memories from childhood were recalled to cue words naming common objects, locations, activities and emotions. Participants recalled their earliest specific memory associated with each word and dated their age at the time of the remembered event. A striking and specific finding emerged: age of earliest memory was…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Children, Memory, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haun, Daniel B. M.; Call, Josep – Cognition, 2009
Recognizing relational similarity relies on the ability to understand that defining object properties might not lie in the objects individually, but in the relations of the properties of various object to each other. This aptitude is highly relevant for many important human skills such as language, reasoning, categorization and understanding…
Descriptors: Evolution, Figurative Language, Animals, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Crookes, Kate; McKone, Elinor – Cognition, 2009
Historically, it was believed the perceptual mechanisms involved in individuating faces developed only very slowly over the course of childhood, and that adult levels of expertise were not reached until well into adolescence. Over the last 10 years, there has been some erosion of this view by demonstrations that all adult-like behavioural…
Descriptors: Maturity (Individuals), Children, Visual Perception, Novels
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Feeney, Aidan; Wilburn, Catherine – Cognition, 2008
Although Sloutsky agrees with our interpretation of our data, he argues that the totality of the evidence supports his claim that children make inductive generalisations on the basis of similarity. Here we take issue with his characterisation of the alternative hypotheses in his informal analysis of the data, and suggest that a thorough Bayesian…
Descriptors: Bayesian Statistics, Logical Thinking, Child Development, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Vasilyeva, Marina; Newcombe, Nora; Duffy, Sean – Cognition, 2008
The present research examines the ability of children as young as 4 years to use models in tasks that require scaling of distance along a single dimension. In Experiment 1, we found that tasks involving models are similar in difficulty to those involving maps that we studied earlier (Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N., & Vasilyeva, M. (1999). Spatial…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Play, Scaling, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gutheil, Grant; Bloom, Paul; Valderrama, Nohemy; Freedman, Rebecca – Cognition, 2004
It is commonly assumed that artifacts are named solely on the basis of properties they currently possess; in particular, their appearance and function. The experiments presented here explore the alternative proposal that the history of an artifact plays some role in how it is named. In three experiments, children between the ages of 4 and 9 years…
Descriptors: Intuition, Children, Adults, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Heyman, Gail D.; Phillips, Ann T.; Gelman, Susan A. – Cognition, 2003
Examined reasoning about physics principles within and across ontological kinds among 5- and 7-year-olds and adults. Found that all age groups tended to appropriately generalize what they learned across ontological kinds. Children assumed that principles learned with reference to one ontological kind were more likely to apply within that kind than…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Abu-Akel, Ahmad; Bailey, Alison L. – Cognition, 2001
Provides a theoretical account of children's success on theory of mind (ToM) tasks and the discrepancies found across different ToM tasks, and examines the role of indexical and symbolic referencing. Found that 4- to 6-year-olds succeeded more on tasks with a high ratio of indexical to symbolic references than on tasks with a high ratio of…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development, Performance Factors
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rousselle, Laurence; Noel, Marie-Pascale – Cognition, 2007
Forty-five children with mathematics learning disabilities, with and without comorbid reading disabilities, were compared to 45 normally achieving peers in tasks assessing basic numerical skills. Children with mathematics disabilities were only impaired when comparing Arabic digits (i.e., symbolic number magnitude) but not when comparing…
Descriptors: Symbols (Mathematics), Reading Difficulties, Mathematics Education, Learning Disabilities
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3