NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Franconeri, Steven L.; Scimeca, Jason M.; Roth, Jessica C.; Helseth, Sarah A.; Kahn, Lauren E. – Cognition, 2012
Visual processing breaks the world into parts and objects, allowing us not only to examine the pieces individually, but also to perceive the relationships among them. There is work exploring how we perceive spatial relationships within structures with existing representations, such as faces, common objects, or prototypical scenes. But strikingly,…
Descriptors: Classroom Techniques, Numeracy, Spatial Ability, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Magnani, Barbara; Pavani, Francesco; Frassinetti, Francesca – Cognition, 2012
The aim of the present study was to explore the spatial organization of auditory time and the effects of the manipulation of spatial attention on such a representation. In two experiments, we asked 28 adults to classify the duration of auditory stimuli as "short" or "long". Stimuli were tones of high or low pitch, delivered left or right of the…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Auditory Stimuli, Attention, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van Dijck, Jean-Philippe; Fias, Wim – Cognition, 2011
Several psychophysical and neuropsychological investigations have suggested that the mental representation of numbers takes the form of a number line along which magnitude is positioned in ascending order according to our reading habits. A longstanding debate is whether this spatial frame is triggered automatically as intrinsic part of the number…
Descriptors: Reading Habits, Neuropsychology, Semantics, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hoover, Merrit A.; Richardson, Daniel C. – Cognition, 2008
People will often look to empty, uninformative locations in the world when trying to recall spoken information. This spatial indexing behaviour occurs when the information had previously been associated with those locations. It remains unclear, however, whether this behaviour is an example of a simple association across perceptual and cognitive…
Descriptors: Memory, Cues, Classification, Animation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shaki, Samuel; Fischer, Martin H. – Cognition, 2008
Small numbers are spontaneously associated with left space and larger numbers with right space (the SNARC effect), for example when classifying numbers by parity. This effect is often attributed to reading habits but a causal link has so far never been documented. We report that bilingual Russian-Hebrew readers show a SNARC effect after reading…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, Reading Habits, Numbers, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Hedges, Larry V.; Corrigan, Bryce; Crawford, L. Elizabeth – Cognition, 2004
Four experiments are reported in which people organize a space hierarchically when they estimate particular locations in that space. Earlier work showed that people subdivide circles into quadrants bounded at the vertical and horizontal axes, biasing their estimates towards prototypical diagonal locations within those spatial categories…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Classification, Spatial Ability, Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Papafragou, Anna; Massey, Christine; Gleitman, Lila – Cognition, 2002
Two studies investigated whether language-specific patterns encoding manner and direction of motion in English and Greek affect adult and child speakers' performance on nonlinguistic motion tasks and linguistic descriptions of these motion events. Although the two linguistic groups differed in linguistic preferences, nonlinguistic task performance…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis, Contrastive Linguistics