NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 15 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rottman, Joshua; Kelemen, Deborah – Cognition, 2012
The traditional cognitive developmental perspective on moral acquisition posits that children actively construct moral beliefs by assessing the negative impacts of antisocial behaviors. This account is not easily applied to actions that are considered immoral despite lacking consequences for others' welfare. We studied the moralization of…
Descriptors: Norms, Moral Values, Moral Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roder, Brigitte; Pagel, Birthe; Heed, Tobias – Cognition, 2013
The integrated use of spatial and temporal information seems to support the separation of two sensory streams. The present study tested whether this facilitation depends on the encoding of sensory stimuli in externally anchored spatial coordinate systems. Fifty-nine children between 5 and 12 years as well as 12 young adults performed a crossmodal…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Cognitive Processes, Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Back, Elisa; Apperly, Ian A. – Cognition, 2010
A recent study by Apperly et al. (2006) found evidence that adults do not automatically infer false beliefs while watching videos that afford such inferences. This method was extended to examine true beliefs, which are sometimes thought to be ascribed by "default" (e.g., Leslie & Thaiss, 1992). Sequences of pictures were presented in which the…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Personality, Inferences, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Connell, Louise; Lynott, Dermot – Cognition, 2010
Recent neuroimaging research has shown that perceptual and conceptual processing share a common, modality-specific neural substrate, while work on modality switching costs suggests that they share some of the same attentional mechanisms. In three experiments, we employed a modality detection task that displayed modality-specific object properties…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Cognitive Development, Language Processing, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Watanabe, Katsumi – Cognition, 2008
To coordinate our actions with those of others, it is crucial to not only choose an appropriate category of action but also to execute it at an appropriate timing. It is widely documented that people tend to unconsciously mimic others' behavior. The present study show that people also tend to modify their movement timing according to others'…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Observation, Motion, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Senju, Atsushi; Csibra, Gergely; Johnson, Mark H. – Cognition, 2008
In four experiments, we investigated whether 9-month-old infants are sensitive to the relationship between gaze direction and object location and whether this sensitivity depends on the presence of communicative cues like eye contact. Infants observed a face, which repeatedly shifted its eyes either toward, or away from, unpredictably appearing…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jordan, Kerry E.; Suanda, Sumarga H.; Brannon, Elizabeth M. – Cognition, 2008
Intersensory redundancy can facilitate animal and human behavior in areas as diverse as rhythm discrimination, signal detection, orienting responses, maternal call learning, and associative learning. In the realm of numerical development, infants show similar sensitivity to numerical differences in both the visual and auditory modalities. Using a…
Descriptors: Infants, Associative Learning, Redundancy, Cognitive Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Needham, Amy; Baillargeon, Renee – Cognition, 1997
Examined infants' use of configural and physical knowledge in segregating three-dimensional adjacent displays. Found that infants do use configural knowledge: they expect similar parts to belong to same unit and dissimilar parts to belong to distinct units. Also found that physical knowledge, such as impenetrability and support, influences their…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Fundamental Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kirkham, Natasha Z.; Slemmer, Jonathan A.; Johnson, Scott P. – Cognition, 2002
Habituated 2-, 5-, and 8-month-olds to visual stimuli following statistically predictable pattern, then showed the familiar pattern alternating with novel sequence of identical stimuli. Found significantly greater interest in novel sequence at all ages. Results support likelihood of domain general statistical learning in infancy and imply that…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Habituation, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Swingley, Daniel; Pinto, John P.; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 1999
Three experiments used a visual fixation technique to examine whether toddlers interpret speech continuously. Found that 24-month-olds had delayed responses when a competing distractor picture's label overlapped phonetically with the target at onset, but not when the pictures' labels rhymed, showing that children monitored speech stream…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Arterberry, Martha E.; Bornstein, Marc H. – Cognition, 2002
Five experiments used a categorization habituation-of-looking paradigm to investigate infants' categorization of animals and vehicles based on static versus dynamic attributes of stimuli (color images versus dynamic point-light displays). Findings showed that 6-month-olds categorize animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic information, and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Development, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Phillips, Ann T.; Wellman, Henry M.; Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Cognition, 2002
Examined in four studies whether and when infants connect information about an actor's affect and perception to their action. Found that 12-month-olds, but not 8-month-olds, recognized that an actor was likely to grasp the object she had visually regarded with positive affect. Replicated findings with 12- and 14-month-olds and with several…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Emotional Response
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carey, Susan; Xu, Fei – Cognition, 2001
Examines evidence that the research community studying infants' object concept and the community concerned with adult object-based attention have been studying the same natural kind. Maintains that the discovery that the object representations of young infants are the same as the object files of mid-level visual cognition has implications for both…
Descriptors: Adults, Attention, Attention Control, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Needham, Amy; Baillargeon, Renee – Cognition, 2000
Summarizes findings on infants' capacity for object segregation. Maintains that infants can use featural and experiential information for segregation and individuation purposes long before 12 months of age. Disputes the claim that formation of object categories awaits early word learning, but acknowledges that language may play a key role in…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Xu, Fei; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2000
Responds to Needham and Baillargeon's criticisms and offers an alternative resolution of the conflicting results between the laboratories regarding abilities of infants less than 12 months to use property/featural information for object individuation. Maintains that kind concepts are acquired as infants approach their first birthday and that…
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Concept Formation