Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 14 |
Verbs | 14 |
Sentences | 5 |
Semantics | 4 |
Sentence Structure | 4 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Eye Movements | 3 |
Grammar | 3 |
Learning Processes | 3 |
Nouns | 3 |
Auditory Stimuli | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 14 |
Author
Aicher, Karen A. | 1 |
Ambridge, Ben | 1 |
Borovsky, Arielle | 1 |
Boster, James S. | 1 |
Bowerman, Melissa | 1 |
Chen, Helen | 1 |
Elman, Jeff | 1 |
Fang, Shin-Yi | 1 |
Feist, Michele I. | 1 |
Ferretti, Todd R. | 1 |
Garvey, Catherine | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 12 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ambridge, Ben; Pine, Julian M.; Rowland, Caroline F. – Cognition, 2012
The present study investigated how children learn that some verbs may appear in the figure-locative but not the ground-locative construction (e.g., "Lisa poured water into the cup"; "*Lisa poured the cup with water"), with some showing the opposite pattern (e.g., "*Bart filled water into the cup"; "Bart filled the cup with water"), and others…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Grammar, Exhibits
Borovsky, Arielle; Kutas, Marta; Elman, Jeff – Cognition, 2010
Humans have the remarkable capacity to learn words from a single instance. The goal of this study was to examine the impact of initial learning context on the understanding of novel word usage using event-related brain potentials. Participants saw known and unknown words in strongly or weakly constraining sentence contexts. After each sentence…
Descriptors: Sentences, Verbs, Vocabulary Development, Learning Processes
Koring, Loes; Mak, Pim; Reuland, Eric – Cognition, 2012
Previous research has found that the single argument of unaccusative verbs (such as "fall") is reactivated during sentence processing, but the argument of agentive verbs (such as "jump") is not ( and ). An open question so far was whether this difference in processing is caused by a difference in thematic roles the verbs assign, or a difference in…
Descriptors: Sentences, Models, Verbs, Syntax
Kukona, Anuenue; Fang, Shin-Yi; Aicher, Karen A.; Chen, Helen; Magnuson, James S. – Cognition, 2011
Several studies have demonstrated that as listeners hear sentences describing events in a scene, their eye movements anticipate upcoming linguistic items predicted by the unfolding relationship between scene and sentence. While this may reflect active prediction based on structural or contextual expectations, the influence of local thematic…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Sentence Structure, Verbs
Feist, Michele I. – Cognition, 2010
The introduction of (Talmy, 1985), (Talmy, 1985) and (Talmy, 2000) typology sparked significant interest in linguistic relativity in the arena of motion language. Through careful analysis of the conflation patterns evident in the language of motion events, Talmy noted that one class of languages, V-languages, tends to encode path along with the…
Descriptors: Verbs, Motion, Languages, Coding
Staub, Adrian – Cognition, 2010
Speakers are known to make subject-verb agreement errors both when a number-mismatching noun intervenes between the head of the subject phrase and the verb (e.g., "*The key to the cabinets are on the table") and in configurations in which there is a number-mismatching noun that does not intervene (e.g., "*The cabinets that the key open are on the…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Verbs, Nouns, Grammar
Imai, Mutsumi; Kita, Sotaro; Nagumo, Miho; Okada, Hiroyuki – Cognition, 2008
Some words are sound-symbolic in that they involve a non-arbitrary relationship between sound and meaning. Here, we report that 25-month-old children are sensitive to cross-linguistically valid sound-symbolic matches in the domain of action and that this sound symbolism facilitates verb learning in young children. We constructed a set of novel…
Descriptors: Verbs, Japanese, Auditory Stimuli, Young Children
Majid, Asifa; Boster, James S.; Bowerman, Melissa – Cognition, 2008
The cross-linguistic investigation of semantic categories has a long history, spanning many disciplines and covering many domains. But the extent to which semantic categories are universal or language-specific remains highly controversial. Focusing on the domain of events involving material destruction ("cutting and breaking" events, for short),…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Linguistics, Classification
Papafragou, Anna; Hulbert, Justin; Trueswell, John – Cognition, 2008
Languages differ in how they encode motion. When describing bounded motion, English speakers typically use verbs that convey information about manner (e.g., "slide", "skip", "walk") rather than path (e.g., "approach", "ascend"), whereas Greek speakers do the opposite. We investigated whether this strong cross-language difference influences how…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Attention, Motion, Visual Perception
Ferretti, Todd R.; Singer, Murray; Patterson, Courtney – Cognition, 2008
We examined how verb factivity influences the ability of readers to detect and resolve the mismatch of receiving false referents in relation to true referents in discourse contexts. Factive verbs (e.g., know), but not nonfactive verbs (believe), entail the truth of their complements. Recent research by Singer [Singer, M. (2006). Verification of…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Sentences, Verbs, Nouns

Garvey, Catherine; And Others – Cognition, 1974
A technique is demonstrated whereby an implicit semantic feature can be related to a grammatical alternative (pronoun-antecedent assignment) and thereby made explicit. It is also demonstrated that pragmatic, syntactic and other semantic features interact in an orderly way with this implicit feature of causality in verbs. (RC)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Language, Pronouns
Marshall, Chloe R.; van der Lely, Heather K. J. – Cognition, 2006
Is past tense production better modelled by a Single Mechanism or a Words and Rules model? We present data concerning a phenomenon that has not been considered by either model--regular past tense verbs with contrasting phonotactics. One set of verbs contains clusters at the inflected verb end that also occur in monomorphemic words…
Descriptors: Verbs, Cognitive Processes, Morphemes, Phonology

Grober, Ellen H.; And Others – Cognition, 1978
Subjects completed sentences of the form NP1 aux V NP2 because (but) Pro...(e.g., John may scold Bill because he...) with a reason or motive for the action described. A basic perceptual strategy was hypothesized to underlie the comprehension of these sentences which have a potentially ambiguous pronoun in the subject position of the subordinate…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Context Clues, Deep Structure, Higher Education

Vigliocco, Gabriella; And Others – Cognition, 1996
Reports four experiments examining subject-verb agreement errors in Spanish and English. Discusses cross-linguistic differences within the framework of the computational model of grammatical encoding proposed by Kempen and Hoenkamp. Suggests that languages differ in the extent to which the selection of the verb is controlled by features on the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comparative Analysis, Discourse Analysis, English