Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 28 |
Descriptor
Infants | 49 |
Language Acquisition | 49 |
Cognitive Development | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Cues | 8 |
Syntax | 8 |
Vocabulary Development | 8 |
Adults | 7 |
Classification | 7 |
Learning Processes | 7 |
Linguistic Input | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 49 |
Author
Gerken, LouAnn | 4 |
Saffran, Jenny R. | 3 |
Werker, Janet F. | 3 |
Akhtar, Nameera | 2 |
Aslin, Richard N. | 2 |
Lidz, Jeffrey | 2 |
Morgan, James L. | 2 |
Naigles, Letitia R. | 2 |
Nazzi, Thierry | 2 |
Pons, Ferran | 2 |
Shi, Rushen | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 48 |
Reports - Research | 33 |
Reports - Evaluative | 8 |
Opinion Papers | 4 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 9 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
France | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Vlach, Haley A.; Johnson, Scott P. – Cognition, 2013
Infants are able to map linguistic labels to referents in the world by tracking co-occurrence probabilities across learning events, a behavior often termed "cross-situational statistical learning." This study builds upon existing research by examining infants' developing ability to aggregate and retrieve word-referent pairings over time. 16- and…
Descriptors: Memory, Infants, Learning, Statistics
Feldman, Naomi H.; Myers, Emily B.; White, Katherine S.; Griffiths, Thomas L.; Morgan, James L. – Cognition, 2013
Infants begin to segment words from fluent speech during the same time period that they learn phonetic categories. Segmented words can provide a potentially useful cue for phonetic learning, yet accounts of phonetic category acquisition typically ignore the contexts in which sounds appear. We present two experiments to show that, contrary to the…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Infants, Cues, Adults
Bergelson, Elika; Swingley, Daniel – Cognition, 2013
Young infants' learning of words for abstract concepts like "all gone" and "eat," in contrast to their learning of more concrete words like "apple" and "shoe," may follow a relatively protracted developmental course. We examined whether infants know such abstract words. Parents named one of two events shown in side-by-side videos while their…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Video Technology, Infants, Vocabulary Development
Bion, Ricardo A. H.; Borovsky, Arielle; Fernald, Anne – Cognition, 2013
When hearing a novel name, children tend to select a novel object rather than a familiar one, a bias known as disambiguation. Using online processing measures with 18-, 24-, and 30-month-olds, we investigate how the development of this bias relates to word learning. Children's proportion of looking time to a novel object after hearing a novel name…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Novels, Vocabulary Development, Infants
Levelt, Clara C. – Cognition, 2012
In a word learning experiment, 14- and 18-month-old infants are tested on their perceptual sensitivity to coda-consonant omissions. The results indicate that 14-month-olds are not sensitive to coda consonant omissions, showing a parallel with the omission of target coda consonants in early child language productions. At 18 months, infants are…
Descriptors: Phonemes, Child Language, Infants, Language Acquisition
Benitez, Viridiana L.; Smith, Linda B. – Cognition, 2012
Expectancy-based localized attention has been shown to promote the formation and retrieval of multisensory memories in adults. Three experiments show that these processes also characterize attention and learning in 16- to 18-month old infants and, moreover, that these processes may play a critical role in supporting early object name learning. The…
Descriptors: Infants, Object Permanence, Prediction, Language Acquisition
Marquis, Alexandra; Shi, Rushen – Cognition, 2012
How do children learn the internal structure of inflected words? We hypothesized that bound functional morphemes begin to be encoded at the preverbal stage, driven by their frequent occurrence with highly variable roots, and that infants in turn use these morphemes to interpret other words with the same inflections. Using a preferential looking…
Descriptors: Semantics, Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Infants
Rasanen, Okko – Cognition, 2011
Word segmentation from continuous speech is a difficult task that is faced by human infants when they start to learn their native language. Several studies indicate that infants might use several different cues to solve this problem, including intonation, linguistic stress, and transitional probabilities between subsequent speech sounds. In this…
Descriptors: Speech, Infants, Cues, Language Acquisition
Martin, Alia; Onishi, Kristine H.; Vouloumanos, Athena – Cognition, 2012
Adult humans recognize that even unfamiliar speech can communicate information between third parties, demonstrating an ability to separate communicative function from linguistic content. We examined whether 12-month-old infants understand that speech can communicate before they understand the meanings of specific words. Specifically, we test the…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, Speech Communication, Age Differences
Lew-Williams, Casey; Saffran, Jenny R. – Cognition, 2012
Infants have been described as "statistical learners" capable of extracting structure (such as words) from patterned input (such as language). Here, we investigated whether prior knowledge influences how infants track transitional probabilities in word segmentation tasks. Are infants biased by prior experience when engaging in sequential…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, Linguistic Input, Prior Learning
Frank, Michael C.; Tenenbaum, Joshua B. – Cognition, 2011
Children learning the inflections of their native language show the ability to generalize beyond the perceptual particulars of the examples they are exposed to. The phenomenon of "rule learning"--quick learning of abstract regularities from exposure to a limited set of stimuli--has become an important model system for understanding generalization…
Descriptors: Native Language, Observation, Models, Language Acquisition
Nazzi, Thierry; Barriere, Isabelle; Goyet, Louise; Kresh, Sarah; Legendre, Geraldine – Cognition, 2011
This study examines French-learning infants' sensitivity to grammatical non-adjacent dependencies involving subject-verb agreement (e.g., "le/les garcons lit/lisent" "the boy(s) read(s)") where number is audible on both the determiner of the subject DP and the agreeing verb, and the dependency is spanning across two syntactic phrases. A further…
Descriptors: Verbs, Grammar, Infants, French
Pons, Ferran; Toro, Juan M. – Cognition, 2010
Recent research has suggested consonants and vowels serve different roles during language processing. While statistical computations are preferentially made over consonants but not over vowels, simple structural generalizations are easily made over vowels but not over consonants. Nevertheless, the origins of this asymmetry are unknown. Here we…
Descriptors: Vowels, Language Tests, Infants, Language Processing
Hochmann, Jean-Remy; Endress, Ansgar D.; Mehler, Jacques – Cognition, 2010
While content words (e.g., 'dog') tend to carry meaning, function words (e.g., 'the') mainly serve syntactic purposes. Here, we ask whether 17-month old infants can use one language-universal cue to identify function word candidates: their high frequency of occurrence. In Experiment 1, infants listened to a series of short, naturally recorded…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Nouns, Infants
Gerken, LouAnn – Cognition, 2010
Previous work demonstrated that 9-month-olds who were familiarized with 3-syllable strings consistent with both a broader (AAB or ABA) and narrower (AA"di" or A"di"A) generalization made only the latter. Because the narrower generalization is a subset of the broader one, any example that is consistent with the broader generalization but not the…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Infants, Decision Making, Generalization