Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Observation | 11 |
Task Analysis | 11 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Cues | 4 |
Experiments | 4 |
Attention | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
Spatial Ability | 3 |
Visual Stimuli | 3 |
Human Body | 2 |
Intention | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 11 |
Author
Adams, Wendy J. | 1 |
Ariga, Atsunori | 1 |
Becchio, Cristina | 1 |
Beran, Michael J. | 1 |
Blank, Hartmut | 1 |
Castiello, Umberto | 1 |
Downing, Paul | 1 |
Elsley, Jane V. | 1 |
Evans, Theodore A. | 1 |
Fernbach, Philip M. | 1 |
Gordon, Lucy | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 11 |
Reports - Research | 10 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Evans, Theodore A.; Beran, Michael J. – Cognition, 2012
Prospective memory (PM) involves forming intentions, retaining those intentions, and later executing those intended responses at the appropriate time. Few studies have investigated this capacity in animals. Monkeys performed a computerized task that assessed their ability to remember to make a particular response if they observed a PM cue embedded…
Descriptors: Memory, Stimuli, Intention, Investigations
Ariga, Atsunori; Lleras, Alejandro – Cognition, 2011
We newly propose that the vigilance decrement occurs because the cognitive control system fails to maintain active the goal of the vigilance task over prolonged periods of time (goal habituation). Further, we hypothesized that momentarily deactivating this goal (via a switch in tasks) would prevent the activation level of the vigilance goal from…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Brain, Task Analysis, Attention
Oeberst, Aileen; Blank, Hartmut – Cognition, 2012
Presenting inconsistent postevent information about a witnessed incident typically decreases the accuracy of memory reports concerning that event (the "misinformation effect"). Surprisingly, the "reversibility" of the effect (after an initial occurrence) has remained largely unexplored. Based on a "memory conversion" theoretical framework and…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Memory, Models, Experiments
Green, Jessica J.; Woldorff, Marty G. – Cognition, 2012
The observation of cueing effects (faster responses for cued than uncued targets) rapidly following centrally-presented arrows has led to the suggestion that arrows trigger rapid automatic shifts of spatial attention. However, these effects have primarily been observed during easy target-detection tasks when both cue and target remain on the…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Intervals, Conflict, Attention
Gozli, Davood G.; West, Greg L.; Pratt, Jay – Cognition, 2012
The present study investigated the mechanisms responsible for the difference between visual processing of stimuli near and far from the observer's hands. The idea that objects near the hands are immediate candidates for action led us to hypothesize that vision near the hands would be biased toward the action-oriented magnocellular visual pathway…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Vision, Visual Perception, Spatial Ability
Taylor, J. Eric T.; Witt, Jessica K.; Grimaldi, Phillip J. – Cognition, 2012
Observed actions are covertly and involuntarily simulated within the observer's motor system. It has been argued that simulation is involved in processing abstract, gestural paintings, as the artist's movements can be simulated by observing static brushstrokes. Though this argument is grounded in theory, empirical research has yet to examine the…
Descriptors: Evidence, Audiences, Artists, Painting (Visual Arts)
Sartori, Luisa; Becchio, Cristina; Castiello, Umberto – Cognition, 2011
Body movement provides a rich source of cues about other people's goals and intentions. In the present research, we investigate how well people can distinguish between different social intentions on the basis of movement information. Participants observed a model reaching toward and grasping a wooden block with the intent to cooperate with a…
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Role, Intention
Parmentier, Fabrice B. R.; Elsley, Jane V.; Ljungberg, Jessica K. – Cognition, 2010
Unexpected events often distract us. In the laboratory, novel auditory stimuli have been shown to capture attention away from a focal visual task and yield specific electrophysiological responses as well as a behavioral cost to performance. Distraction is thought to follow ineluctably from the sound's low probability of occurrence or, put more…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Probability, Computer Software, Laboratories
Sloman, Steven A.; Fernbach, Philip M.; Hagmayer, York – Cognition, 2010
The paper sets out to reveal conditions enabling diagnostic self-deception, people's tendency to deceive themselves about the diagnostic value of their own actions. We characterize different types of self-deception in terms of the distinction between intervention and observation in causal reasoning. One type arises when people intervene but choose…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Intelligence, Educational Policy, Deception
Adams, Wendy J. – Cognition, 2008
Faced with highly complex and ambiguous visual input, human observers must rely on prior knowledge and assumptions to efficiently determine the structure of their surroundings. One of these assumptions is the "light-from-above" prior. In the absence of explicit light-source information, the visual system assumes that the light-source is roughly…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Prior Learning, Cognitive Processes, Observation
Morrison, India; Poliakoff, Ellen; Gordon, Lucy; Downing, Paul – Cognition, 2007
How does seeing a painful event happening to someone else influence the observer's own motor system? To address this question, we measured simple reaction times following videos showing noxious or innocuous implements contacting corporeal or noncorporeal objects. Key releases in a go/nogo task were speeded, and key presses slowed, after subjects…
Descriptors: Observation, Psychomotor Skills, Pain, Reaction Time