Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 5 |
Descriptor
Language Processing | 6 |
Syntax | 6 |
Task Analysis | 6 |
Verbs | 3 |
Eye Movements | 2 |
Grammar | 2 |
Linguistic Theory | 2 |
Sentences | 2 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 1 |
Computational Linguistics | 1 |
Context Effect | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 6 |
Author
Arunachalam, Sudha | 1 |
Barner, David | 1 |
Bernolet, Sarah | 1 |
Hartsuiker, Robert J. | 1 |
Kaiser, Elsi | 1 |
Knecht, Stefan | 1 |
Monaghan, Padraic | 1 |
Snedeker, Jesse | 1 |
Sturt, Patrick | 1 |
Trueswell, John C. | 1 |
Wagner, Laura | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 4 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Arunachalam, Sudha; Waxman, Sandra R. – Cognition, 2010
When toddlers view an event while hearing a novel verb, the verb's syntactic context has been shown to help them identify its meaning. The current work takes this finding one step further to reveal that even in the absence of an accompanying event, syntactic information supports toddlers' identification of verb meaning. Two-year-olds were first…
Descriptors: Sentences, Verbs, Syntax, Toddlers
Bernolet, Sarah; Hartsuiker, Robert J. – Cognition, 2010
In a corpus analysis of spontaneous speech Jaeger and Snider (2007) found that the strength of structural priming is correlated with verb alternation bias. This finding is consistent with an implicit learning account of syntactic priming: because the implicit learning model implemented by Chang (2002), Chang, Dell, and Bock (2006), and Chang,…
Descriptors: Speech, Verbs, Computational Linguistics, Syntax
Barner, David; Wagner, Laura; Snedeker, Jesse – Cognition, 2008
What does mass-count syntax contribute to the interpretation of noun phrases (NPs), and how much of NP meaning is contributed by lexical items alone? Many have argued that count syntax specifies reference to countable individuals (e.g., "cats") while mass syntax specifies reference to unindividuated entities (e.g., "water"). We evaluated this…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Syntax, Phrase Structure
de Vries, Meinou H.; Monaghan, Padraic; Knecht, Stefan; Zwitserlood, Pienie – Cognition, 2008
Embedded hierarchical structures, such as "the rat the cat ate was brown", constitute a core generative property of a natural language theory. Several recent studies have reported learning of hierarchical embeddings in artificial grammar learning (AGL) tasks, and described the functional specificity of Broca's area for processing such structures.…
Descriptors: Syntax, Memory, Natural Language Processing, Grammar
Sturt, Patrick – Cognition, 2007
Participant's eye-movements were recorded while they read locally ambiguous sentences. Evidence for processing difficulty was found when the interpretation of the initially preferred misanalysis clashed with that of the globally correct analysis, demonstrating the persistence of the earlier interpretation. Processing difficulty associated with the…
Descriptors: Semantics, Persistence, Sentences, Language Processing
Kaiser, Elsi; Trueswell, John C. – Cognition, 2004
On-line comprehension studies of flexible word-order languages find that noncanonical ("scrambled") structures induce more difficulty than canonical structures [e.g., Hyona & Hujanen, "Q. J. Exp. Psychol." 50A (1997) 841-858], with this difference being attributed to the structural complexity/infrequency of these forms. However, by presenting…
Descriptors: Syntax, Discourse Modes, Finno Ugric Languages, Language Processing