Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 23 |
Descriptor
Semantics | 23 |
Semiotics | 23 |
Cognitive Processes | 11 |
Experiments | 9 |
Language Processing | 9 |
Word Recognition | 7 |
Nouns | 5 |
Recall (Psychology) | 5 |
Visual Stimuli | 5 |
Children | 4 |
Task Analysis | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognition | 23 |
Author
Afonso, Olivia | 1 |
Alario, F. -Xavier. | 1 |
Ambridge, Ben | 1 |
Aviles, Alberto | 1 |
Badets, Arnaud | 1 |
Barner, David | 1 |
Bekkering, H. | 1 |
Bi, Yanchao | 1 |
Blackford, Trevor | 1 |
Bolte, Annette | 1 |
Campanella, Fabio | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 23 |
Reports - Research | 19 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Education Level
Preschool Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Davidson, Kathryn; Eng, Kortney; Barner, David – Cognition, 2012
We tested the hypothesis that, when children learn to correctly count sets, they make a semantic induction about the meanings of their number words. We tested the logical understanding of number words in 84 children that were classified as "cardinal-principle knowers" by the criteria set forth by Wynn (1992). Results show that these children often…
Descriptors: Evidence, Semantics, Numbers, Logical Thinking
Blackford, Trevor; Holcomb, Phillip J.; Grainger, Jonathan; Kuperberg, Gina R. – Cognition, 2012
We measured Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) and naming times to picture targets preceded by masked words (stimulus onset asynchrony: 80 ms) that shared one of three different types of relationship with the names of the pictures: (1) Identity related, in which the prime was the name of the picture ("socks"--[picture of socks]), (2) Phonemic Onset…
Descriptors: Priming, Phonemics, Semantics, Cognitive Processes
Roland, Douglas; Yun, Hongoak; Koenig, Jean-Pierre; Mauner, Gail – Cognition, 2012
The effects of word predictability and shared semantic similarity between a target word and other words that could have taken its place in a sentence on language comprehension are investigated using data from a reading time study, a sentence completion study, and linear mixed-effects regression modeling. We find that processing is facilitated if…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Sentences, Semantics, Probability
Gillespie, Maureen; Pearlmutter, Neal J. – Cognition, 2011
Two subject-verb agreement error elicitation studies tested the hierarchical feature-passing account of agreement computation in production and three timing-based alternatives: linear distance to the head noun, semantic integration, and a combined effect of both (a scope of planning account). In Experiment 1, participants completed subject noun…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Semantics, Verbs, Nouns
Bi, Yanchao; Yu, Xi; Geng, Jingyi; Alario, F. -Xavier. – Cognition, 2010
The interface between the conceptual and lexical systems was investigated in a word production setting. We tested the effects of two conceptual dimensions--semantic category and visual shape--on the selection of Chinese nouns and classifiers. Participants named pictures with nouns ("rope") or classifier-noun phrases ("one-"classifier"-rope") in…
Descriptors: Semantics, Nouns, Cognitive Processes, Semiotics
Sabbagh, Mark A.; Shafman, Dana – Cognition, 2009
Preschool children typically do not learn words from ignorant or unreliable speakers. Here, we examined the mechanism by which these learning failures occur by modifying the comprehension test procedure that measures word learning. Following lexical training by a knowledgeable or ignorant speaker, 48 preschool-aged children were asked either a…
Descriptors: Semantics, Preschool Children, Semiotics, Coding
van Elk, M.; van Schie, H.T.; Bekkering, H. – Cognition, 2009
In the present study, we investigated whether the preparation of an unusual action with an object (e.g. bringing a cup towards the eye) could selectively overrule long-term semantic representations. In the first experiment it was found that unusual action intentions activated short-term semantic goal representations, rather than long-term…
Descriptors: Semantics, Word Processing, Semiotics, Intention
Campanella, Fabio; Shallice, Tim – Cognition, 2011
While many behavioural studies on refractory phenomena in lexical/semantic access have focused on the mechanisms involved in the oral production of names, comprehension tasks have been almost exclusively used in neuropsychological studies on brain damaged patients. We report the results of two experiments on healthy participants conducted by means…
Descriptors: Semantics, Serial Ordering, Patients, Brain
Badets, Arnaud; Pesenti, Mauro – Cognition, 2010
Communication, language and conceptual knowledge related to concrete objects may rely on the sensory-motor systems from which they emerge. How abstract concepts can emerge from these systems is however still unknown. Here we report a functional interaction between a specific meaningful finger movement, such as a finger grip closing, and a concept…
Descriptors: Semantics, Scientific Concepts, Interaction, Semiotics
Verdonschot, Rinus G.; La Heij, Wido; Schiller, Niels O. – Cognition, 2010
The process of reading aloud bare nouns in alphabetic languages is immune to semantic context effects from pictures. This is accounted for by assuming that words in alphabetic languages can be read aloud relatively fast through a sub-lexical grapheme-phoneme conversion (GPC) route or by a direct route from orthography to word form. We examined…
Descriptors: Semantics, Scripts, Semiotics, Reading Aloud to Others
Nation, Kate; Cocksey, Joanne – Cognition, 2009
Two experiments assessed whether 7-year-old children activate semantic information from sub-word orthography. Children made category decisions to visually-presented words, some of which contained an embedded word (e.g., "hip" in s"hip"). In Experiment 1 children were slower and less accurate to classify words if they contained an embedded word…
Descriptors: Phonology, Semantics, Semiotics, Word Recognition
Soto, David; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Cognition, 2009
We present data indicating that visual awareness for a basic perceptual feature (colour) can be influenced by the relation between the feature and the semantic properties of the stimulus. We examined semantic interference from the meaning of a colour word ("RED") on simple colour (ink related) detection responses in a patient with simultagnosia…
Descriptors: Semantics, Semiotics, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli
Marsh, John E.; Hughes, Robert W.; Jones, Dylan M. – Cognition, 2009
Distraction by irrelevant background sound of visually-based cognitive tasks illustrates the vulnerability of attentional selectivity across modalities. Four experiments centred on auditory distraction during tests of memory for visually-presented semantic information. Meaningful irrelevant speech disrupted the free recall of semantic…
Descriptors: Semantics, Semiotics, Memory, Attention
Dunabeitia, Jon Andoni; Aviles, Alberto; Afonso, Olivia; Scheepers, Christoph; Carreiras, Manuel – Cognition, 2009
In the present visual-world experiment, participants were presented with visual displays that included a target item that was a semantic associate of an abstract or a concrete word. This manipulation allowed us to test a basic prediction derived from the qualitatively different representational framework that supports the view of different…
Descriptors: Semantics, Vocabulary Development, Semiotics, Models
You, Wenping; Chen, Baoguo; Dunlap, Susan – Cognition, 2009
Frequency trajectory is a better measure to investigate age-limited learning effects than age of acquisition (AoA) ratings (Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). "Age of acquisition effects in word reading and other tasks." "Journal of Memory and Language, 47"(1), 1-29). The current study uses frequency trajectory as a variable to investigate…
Descriptors: Semantics, Written Language, Character Recognition, Chinese
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2