Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Adults | 14 |
Cognitive Development | 14 |
Children | 7 |
Age Differences | 6 |
Comparative Analysis | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Experiments | 4 |
Child Development | 3 |
Evaluation Methods | 3 |
Preschool Children | 3 |
Thinking Skills | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 14 |
Author
Amsel, Eric | 1 |
Brock, Susan | 1 |
Burgund, E. Darcy | 1 |
Chandler, Kacey | 1 |
Chater, Nick | 1 |
Duemmler, Thomas | 1 |
Ellefson, Michelle R. | 1 |
Fein, Deborah | 1 |
Gattis, Merideth | 1 |
Hayes, Rachel A. | 1 |
Jaffery, Gul | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 14 |
Reports - Research | 14 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pillow, Bradford H.; Pearson, RaeAnne M. – Cognitive Development, 2012
In three studies, 5-10-year-old children and an adult comparison group judged another's certainty in making inductive inferences and guesses. Participants observed a puppet make strong inductions, weak inductions, and guesses. Participants either had no information about the correctness of the puppet's conclusion, knew that the puppet was correct,…
Descriptors: Puppetry, Logical Thinking, Inferences, Children
Sakkalou, Elena; Gattis, Merideth – Cognitive Development, 2012
Two studies were conducted to examine infants' ability to discern intentions from lexical and prosodic cues. Two groups of 14-18-month-olds participated in these studies. In both studies, infants watched an adult perform a sequence of two-step actions on novel toys that produced an end-result. In the first study actions were marked intentionally…
Descriptors: Infants, Cues, Intention, Adults
Tek, Saime; Jaffery, Gul; Swensen, Lauren; Fein, Deborah; Naigles, Letitia R. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Previous research has demonstrated that visual properties of objects can affect shape-based categorization in a novel-name extension task; however, we still do not know how a relationship between visual properties of objects affects judgments in a novel-name extension task. We examined effects of increased visual similarity among the target and…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Cognitive Development, Visual Stimuli, Adults
Duemmler, Thomas; Schoeberl, Petra; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Cognitive Development, 2008
Adults assess precisely the center of mass (CM) of single-bodied objects and choose grasping points that span an axis through or close to the center of mass when lifting them. The present study examined how children and adults assess the CM when lifting two-bodied objects (barbells) and how this ability develops. Four- to five-year olds, 6- to…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Adults, Children, Cognitive Development
Kim, Sunae; Kalish, Charles W. – Cognitive Development, 2009
Ownership is not a "natural" property of objects, but is determined by human intentions. Facts about who owns what may be altered by appropriate decisions. However, young children often deny the efficacy of transfer decisions, asserting that original owners retain rights to their property. In Experiment 1, 4-5-year-old and 7-8-year-old children…
Descriptors: Ownership, Intention, Children, Age Differences
Hayes, Rachel A.; Slater, Alan M.; Longmore, Christopher A. – Cognitive Development, 2009
Nine-month-olds can respond to a change in rhyme when the conditioned head turn procedure is used [Hayes, R. A., Slater, A., & Brown, E. (2000). "Infants' ability to categorise on the basis of rhyme." "Cognitive Development, 15," 405-419]. However, it is not known whether infants are detecting the change in vowel, the change in coda, or both. In…
Descriptors: Vowels, Infants, Rhyme, Cognitive Development
Shtulman, Andrew – Cognitive Development, 2009
The ability to differentiate possible events from impossible ones is an invaluable skill when reasoning about claims that transcend the perceptual evidence at hand, yet preschool-aged children do not readily make this differentiation when reasoning about physically extraordinary events [Shtulman, A., & Carey, S. (2007). "Improbable or impossible?…
Descriptors: Adults, Child Development, Preschool Children, Cognitive Development
Burgund, E. Darcy – Cognitive Development, 2009
Repetition priming refers to the facilitation of stimulus processing due to prior processing of the same or similar stimulus, and is one of the most primitive ways in which experience and practice can affect performance. Previous studies have produced contradictory results regarding the stability of repetition priming across development. Drawing…
Descriptors: Reading Ability, Priming, Experiments, Age Differences
Ellefson, Michelle R.; Shapiron, Laura R.; Chater, Nick – Cognitive Development, 2006
Switching between tasks produces decreases in performance as compared to repeating the same task. Asymmetrical switch costs occur when switching between two tasks of unequal difficulty. This asymmetry occurs because the cost is greater when switching to the less difficult task than when switching to the more difficult task. Various theories about…
Descriptors: Children, Difficulty Level, Adults, Age Differences
Richert, Rebekah A.; Lillard, Angeline S. – Cognitive Development, 2004
Discriminating what is pretense from what is real is a fundamental problem in development. Research has addressed the proficiency with which adults and children discriminate between play fighting and real fighting, and yet none (to our knowledge) has investigated discrimination of other kinds of pretense and real acts. In addition, little is known…
Descriptors: Cues, Cognitive Development, Social Cognition, Children

Rosser, Rosemary A.; Chandler, Kacey – Cognitive Development, 1995
Examined how children's and adults' initial conceptions of objects and space influence predictions about the physical world, but lead the naive person to misconstrue a dynamic event. Found that participants proficiently anticipated where an oscillating screen would contact a hidden object, but underestimated the distance until contact.…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Depth Perception

Sophian, Catherine; Wood, Amy – Cognitive Development, 1996
Adapted Keil's predictability method to examine adults' and preschoolers' conceptions of numbers, focusing on the ontological distinction between numbers and sets of objects. Found that children, like adults, attribute spatial-arrangement properties to collections much more than to numbers, although both are considered to have quantitative…
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Structures, Concept Formation

Scholnick, Ellin Kofsky; Wing, Clara S. – Cognitive Development, 1995
Compared the use of conditional logic in adult-adult and adult-child conversation. Results indicated that conversation patterns and inferences were similar except that children made fewer independent inferences and shifts in taxonomic level and responded more frequently to socially controlling statements than did adults. (AA)
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Adults, Age Differences, Child Development

Amsel, Eric; Brock, Susan – Cognitive Development, 1996
Examined developmental differences in evidence evaluation skills among school children, non-college educated adults, and college students, utilizing plant growth variables. Found that children were more strongly influenced by prior beliefs and missing data than were the two adult groups. Age and educational differences were found in the…
Descriptors: Adults, Beliefs, Causal Models, Children