NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 49 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Aschersleben, Gisa; Henning, Anne; Daum, Moritz M. – Cognitive Development, 2013
Research on early physical reasoning has shown surprising discontinuities in developmental trajectories. Infants possess some skills that seem to disappear and then re-emerge in childhood. It has been suggested that prediction skills required in search tasks might cause these discontinuities (Keen, 2003). We tested 3.5- to 5-year-olds'…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Prediction, Preschool Children, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pillow, Bradford H.; Pearson, RaeAnne M. – Cognitive Development, 2012
In three studies, 5-10-year-old children and an adult comparison group judged another's certainty in making inductive inferences and guesses. Participants observed a puppet make strong inductions, weak inductions, and guesses. Participants either had no information about the correctness of the puppet's conclusion, knew that the puppet was correct,…
Descriptors: Puppetry, Logical Thinking, Inferences, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Nguyen, Simone P.; Gelman, A. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Four studies examined the role of generic language in facilitating 4- and 5-year-old children's ability to cross-classify. Participants were asked to classify an item into a familiar (taxonomic or script) category, then cross-classify it into a novel (script or taxonomic) category with the help of a clue expressed in either generic or specific…
Descriptors: Classification, Generalization, Children, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Westermann, Gert; Mareschal, Denis – Cognitive Development, 2012
Computational models are tools for testing mechanistic theories of learning and development. Formal models allow us to instantiate theories of cognitive development in computer simulations. Model behavior can then be compared to real performance. Connectionist models, loosely based on neural information processing, have been successful in…
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Cognitive Development, Computation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frick, Andrea; Newcombe, Nora S. – Cognitive Development, 2012
Spatial scaling is an integral aspect of many spatial tasks that involve symbol-to-referent correspondences (e.g., map reading, drawing). In this study, we asked 3-6-year-olds and adults to locate objects in a two-dimensional spatial layout using information from a second spatial representation (map). We examined how scaling factor and reference…
Descriptors: Scaling, Spatial Ability, Toddlers, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Recchia, Holly E.; Brehl, Beverly A.; Wainryb, Cecilia – Cognitive Development, 2012
The goal of this study was to investigate children's descriptions and evaluations of their reasons for leaving others out of a peer group. A total of 84 children (divided into 7-, 11-, and 17-year-old age groups) provided a narrative account of a time they excluded a peer and were subsequently asked to evaluate their reasons for exclusion. With…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Peer Groups, Cognitive Development, Moral Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Garon, Nancy M.; Longard, Julie; Bryson, Susan E.; Moore, Chris – Cognitive Development, 2012
This study explored factors underlying preschoolers' ability to make future-oriented choices. In a delay-of-gratification choice task, quantity and visibility of the reward was systematically varied. Participants included 90 typically developing children aged 2-4 years. Children made more choices to delay gratification as the quantity of the…
Descriptors: Delay of Gratification, Age Differences, Rewards, Decision Making
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Grazzani, Ilaria; Ornaghi, Veronica – Cognitive Development, 2012
This study investigates the relationship between mental-state language and theory of mind in primary school children. The participants were 110 primary school students (mean age = 9 years and 7 months; SD = 12.7 months). They were evenly divided by gender and belonged to two age groups (8- and 10-year-olds). Linguistic, metacognitive and cognitive…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Verbal Ability, Cognitive Development, Primary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Freitag, Claudia; Schwarzer, Gudrun – Cognitive Development, 2011
Three experiments examined 3- and 5-year-olds' recognition of faces in constant and varied emotional expressions. Children were asked to identify repeatedly presented target faces, distinguishing them from distractor faces, during an immediate recognition test and during delayed assessments after 10 min and one week. Emotional facial expression…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Nonverbal Communication, Psychological Patterns, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Friedman, William J. – Cognitive Development, 2011
Recent research on children's thinking about the future has taken multiple directions, many of which are illustrated in the contributions to this special issue. In this commentary the topic is discussed in the context of research on children's understanding of time, and some of the adaptive challenges of thinking about the future are considered.…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Research, Children, Thinking Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lemaire, Patrick; Lecacheur, Mireille – Cognitive Development, 2011
Third, fifth, and seventh graders selected the best strategy (rounding up or rounding down) for estimating answers to two-digit addition problems. Executive function measures were collected for each individual. Data showed that (a) children's skill at both strategy selection and execution improved with age and (b) increased efficiency in executive…
Descriptors: Grade 3, Grade 5, Grade 7, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lahat, Ayelet; Helwig, Charles C.; Zelazo, Philip David – Cognitive Development, 2012
Moral and conventional violations are usually judged differently: Only moral violations are treated as independent of social rules. To investigate the cognitive processing involved in the development of this distinction, undergraduates (N = 34), adolescents (N = 34), and children (N = 14) read scenarios presented on a computer that had 1 of 3…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Undergraduate Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McCormack, Teresa; Hanley, Mary – Cognitive Development, 2011
Four- and five-year-olds completed two sets of tasks that involved reasoning about the temporal order in which events had occurred in the past or were to occur in the future. Four-year-olds succeeded on the tasks that involved reasoning about the order of past events but not those that involved reasoning about the order of future events, whereas…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Children, Preschool Children, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Russell, James; Cheke, Lucy G.; Clayton, Nicola S.; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Cognitive Development, 2011
We analyze theoretical differences between conceptualist and minimalist approaches to episodic processing in young children. The "episodic-like" minimalism of Clayton and Dickinson (1998) is a species of the latter. We asked whether an "episodic-like" task (structurally similar to ones used by Clayton and Dickinson) in which participants had to…
Descriptors: Young Children, Internet, Child Development, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van den Bos, Wouter; Westenberg, Michiel; van Dijk, Eric; Crone, Eveline A. – Cognitive Development, 2010
We investigate the development of two types of prosocial behavior, trust and reciprocity, as defined using a game-theoretical task that allows investigation of real-time social interaction, among 4 age groups from 9 to 25 years. By manipulating the possible outcome alternatives, we could distinguish among important determinants of trust and…
Descriptors: Prosocial Behavior, Trust (Psychology), Late Adolescents, Adolescents
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4