Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Control Groups | 2 |
Computation | 1 |
Computer Assisted Testing | 1 |
Elementary School Students | 1 |
Experimental Groups | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Inhibition | 1 |
Learning Strategies | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 2 |
Author
Alibali, Martha W. | 1 |
Borst, Gregoire | 1 |
Cassotti, Mathieu | 1 |
Fischer, Allison D. | 1 |
Houde, Olivier | 1 |
Phillips, Karin M. O. | 1 |
Pineau, Arlette | 1 |
Poirel, Nicolas | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Borst, Gregoire; Poirel, Nicolas; Pineau, Arlette; Cassotti, Mathieu; Houde, Olivier – Cognitive Development, 2012
We investigated whether success in number-conservation and class-inclusion tasks relies on a general ability to inhibit misleading strategies. Two groups of 10-year-olds performed inter-task priming between computerized versions of class-inclusion and number-conservation tasks (Experiment 1). In one group, the class-inclusion task served as a…
Descriptors: Priming, Cognitive Development, Inhibition, Numbers
Alibali, Martha W.; Phillips, Karin M. O.; Fischer, Allison D. – Cognitive Development, 2009
Children sometimes solve problems incorrectly because they fail to represent key features of the problems. One potential source of improvements in children's problem representations is learning new problem-solving strategies. Ninety-one 3rd- and 4th-grade students solved mathematical equivalence problems (e.g., 3+4+6=3+__) and completed a…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Control Groups, Problem Solving, Learning Strategies