Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 11 |
Intention | 11 |
Cues | 4 |
Preschool Children | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Child Development | 3 |
Memory | 3 |
Young Children | 3 |
Adults | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Cognitive Ability | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Development | 11 |
Author
Gattis, Merideth | 2 |
Kliegel, Matthias | 2 |
Ackerman, Rakefet | 1 |
Aureli, Tiziana | 1 |
Behne, Tanya | 1 |
Carpenter, Malinda | 1 |
Charman, Tony | 1 |
Fortuna, Alexandra | 1 |
Genco, Maria | 1 |
Grafenhain, Maria | 1 |
Heyes, Celilia | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 11 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Sakkalou, Elena; Gattis, Merideth – Cognitive Development, 2012
Two studies were conducted to examine infants' ability to discern intentions from lexical and prosodic cues. Two groups of 14-18-month-olds participated in these studies. In both studies, infants watched an adult perform a sequence of two-step actions on novel toys that produced an end-result. In the first study actions were marked intentionally…
Descriptors: Infants, Cues, Intention, Adults
Saylor, Megan M.; Sabbagh, Mark A.; Fortuna, Alexandra; Troseth, Georgene – Cognitive Development, 2009
In two studies, we investigated preschoolers' ability to use others' preferences to learn names for things. Two studies demonstrated that preschool children make smart use of others' preferences. In the first study, preschool children only used information about others' preferences when they were clearly linked to referential intentions. The…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Cognitive Development, Learning Strategies, Intention
Over, Harriet; Gattis, Merideth – Cognitive Development, 2010
Using an elicited imitation paradigm, we investigated whether young children imitate the communicative intentions behind speech. Previous research using elicited imitation has shown that children tend to correct ungrammatical sentences. This finding is usually interpreted as evidence that children, like adults, remember and reproduce the gist of…
Descriptors: Sentences, Imitation, Intention, Language Processing
Aureli, Tiziana; Perucchini, Paola; Genco, Maria – Cognitive Development, 2009
Two tasks were administered to 40 children aged from 16 to 20 months (mean age = 18;1), to evaluate children's understanding of declarative and informative intention [Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. "Developmental Science", 8, 492-499;…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Nonverbal Communication, Intention, Cognitive Ability
Koriat, Asher; Ackerman, Rakefet; Lockl, Kathrin; Schneider, Wolfgang – Cognitive Development, 2009
A previous study with adults [Koriat, A. (2008a). "Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition." "Memory & Cognition," 36, 416-428] established a correlation between learning and remembering: items requiring more trials to acquisition (TTA) were less likely to be recalled than those…
Descriptors: Heuristics, Metacognition, Memory, Grade 4
Grafenhain, Maria; Behne, Tanya; Carpenter, Malinda; Tomasello, Michael – Cognitive Development, 2009
We investigated whether infants comprehend others' nonverbal communicative intentions directed to a third person, in an "overhearing" context. An experimenter addressed an assistant and indicated a hidden toy's location by either gazing ostensively or pointing to the location for her. In a matched control condition, the experimenter performed…
Descriptors: Cues, Interpersonal Communication, Infants, Comprehension
Kim, Sunae; Kalish, Charles W. – Cognitive Development, 2009
Ownership is not a "natural" property of objects, but is determined by human intentions. Facts about who owns what may be altered by appropriate decisions. However, young children often deny the efficacy of transfer decisions, asserting that original owners retain rights to their property. In Experiment 1, 4-5-year-old and 7-8-year-old children…
Descriptors: Ownership, Intention, Children, Age Differences
Rendell, Peter G.; Vella, Melissa J.; Kliegel, Matthias; Terrett, Gill – Cognitive Development, 2009
To date, little work has been done investigating prospective memory in children, particularly using a delay-execute paradigm. Two experiments were conducted to investigate this issue with children aged 5-11 years. While playing a computer driving game, children's ability to carry out a delayed intention either immediately a target cue appeared or…
Descriptors: Intention, Children, Memory, Memorization
Sobel, David M. – Cognitive Development, 2007
Two experiments investigated preschoolers' understanding of the relation between pretending and intentional action. In Experiment 1, both 3- and 4-year olds recognized that characters whose actions were intended as pretense were pretending. However, children also judged that characters whose actions gave them the appearance of an entity…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Intention, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Kliegel, Matthias; Jager, Theodor – Cognitive Development, 2007
The present study investigated event-based prospective memory in five age groups of preschoolers (i.e., 2-, 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds). Applying a laboratory-controlled prospective memory procedure, the data showed that event-based prospective memory performance improves across the preschool years, at least between 3 and 6 years of age. However,…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Intention, Preschool Children, Young Children
Huang, Chi-Tai; Heyes, Celilia; Charman, Tony – Cognitive Development, 2006
To clarify the nature of the social cognitive skills involved in preschoolers' reenactment of actions on objects, we studied 31- and 41-month-old children's reenactment of intended acts (''failed attempts'') in Meltzoff's [Meltzoff, A. N. (1995)]. Understanding the intentions of others: Reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children.…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Child Behavior, Imitation, Object Manipulation