Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 2 |
Figurative Language | 2 |
Adolescents | 1 |
Adults | 1 |
Aging (Individuals) | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Context Effect | 1 |
Efficiency | 1 |
English | 1 |
Language Processing | 1 |
Models | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Cognitive Science | 2 |
Author
Burns, Patrick | 1 |
Caruso, Eugene M. | 1 |
Dagerman, Karen Stevens | 1 |
Harm, Michael W. | 1 |
Jaroslawska, Agnieszka J. | 1 |
MacDonald, Maryellen C. | 1 |
McCormack, Teresa | 1 |
O'Connor, Patrick A. | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Burns, Patrick; McCormack, Teresa; Jaroslawska, Agnieszka J.; O'Connor, Patrick A.; Caruso, Eugene M. – Cognitive Science, 2019
Human languages typically employ a variety of spatial metaphors for time (e.g., "I'm looking forward to the weekend"). The metaphorical grounding of time in space is also evident in gesture. The gestures that are performed when talking about time bolster the view that people sometimes think about regions of time as if they were locations…
Descriptors: Time, Nonverbal Communication, Children, Adolescents
Dagerman, Karen Stevens; MacDonald, Maryellen C.; Harm, Michael W. – Cognitive Science, 2006
Older and younger adults' abilities to use context information rapidly during ambiguity resolution were investigated. In Experiments 1 and 2, younger and older adults heard ambiguous words (e.g., fires) in sentences where the preceding context supported either the less frequent or more frequent meaning of the word. Both age groups showed good…
Descriptors: Young Adults, Older Adults, Sentences, Simulation