NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Craycraft, Nicole N.; Brown-Schmidt, Sarah – Cognitive Science, 2018
The common ground that conversational partners share is thought to form the basic context for language use. According to the classic view, inferences about common ground, or mutual knowledge, are guided by beliefs about the physical, cognitive, and attentional states of one's communicative partners. Here, we provide a first test of the…
Descriptors: Interpersonal Communication, Attention, Social Cognition, Audience Awareness
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Murphy, Gregory L.; Bosch, David A.; Kim, ShinWoo – Cognitive Science, 2017
Six experiments investigated variables predicted to influence subjects' tendency to classify items by a single property ("rule-based" responding) instead of overall similarity, following the paradigm of Norenzayan et al. (2002, "Cognitive Science"), who found that European Americans tended to give more "logical"…
Descriptors: Preferences, Classification, Predictor Variables, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dufour, Sophie; Nguyen, Noël – Cognitive Science, 2017
In this study, we examined whether the lexical competition process embraced by most models of spoken word recognition is sensitive to talker-specific information. We used a lexical decision task and a long lag priming experiment in which primes and targets sharing all phonemes except the last one (e.g., /bagaR/"fight" vs.…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Word Recognition, Lexicology, Competition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Kleijn, Roy; Kachergis, George; Hommel, Bernhard – Cognitive Science, 2018
Sequential action makes up the bulk of human daily activity, and yet much remains unknown about how people learn such actions. In one motor learning paradigm, the serial reaction time (SRT) task, people are taught a consistent sequence of button presses by cueing them with the next target response. However, the SRT task only records keypress…
Descriptors: Sequential Learning, Reinforcement, Psychomotor Skills, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Devereux, Barry J.; Taylor, Kirsten I.; Randall, Billi; Geertzen, Jeroen; Tyler, Lorraine K. – Cognitive Science, 2016
Understanding spoken words involves a rapid mapping from speech to conceptual representations. One distributed feature-based conceptual account assumes that the statistical characteristics of concepts' features--the number of concepts they occur in ("distinctiveness/sharedness") and likelihood of co-occurrence ("correlational…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Semantics, Concept Mapping, Statistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Meilinger, Tobias; Knauff, Markus; Bulthoff, Heinrich H. – Cognitive Science, 2008
This study examines the working memory systems involved in human wayfinding. In the learning phase, 24 participants learned two routes in a novel photorealistic virtual environment displayed on a 220 degrees screen while they were disrupted by a visual, a spatial, a verbal, or--in a control group--no secondary task. In the following wayfinding…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Short Term Memory, Virtual Classrooms, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Borreggine, Kristin L.; Kaschak, Michael P. – Cognitive Science, 2006
When participants are asked to make sensibility judgments on sentences that describe action toward the body (i.e., "Mark dealt the cards to you") or away from the body (i.e., "You dealt the cards to Mark"), they are faster to respond when the response requires an arm movement in the same direction as the action described by the sentence. This…
Descriptors: Sentences, Responses, Experiments, Motor Reactions