NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 10 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sidhu, David M.; Khachatoorian, Nareg; Vigliocco, Gabriella – Cognitive Science, 2023
Iconicity refers to a resemblance between word form and meaning. Previous work has shown that iconic words are learned earlier and processed faster. Here, we examined whether iconic words are recognized better on a recognition memory task. We also manipulated the level at which items were encoded--with a focus on either their meaning or their…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Language Usage, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ilona Bass; Cristian Espinoza; Elizabeth Bonawitz; Tomer D. Ullman – Cognitive Science, 2024
When people make decisions, they act in a way that is either automatic ("rote"), or more thoughtful ("reflective"). But do people notice when "others" are behaving in a rote way, and do they care? We examine the detection of rote behavior and its consequences in U.S. adults, focusing specifically on pedagogy and…
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Learning Processes, Rote Learning, Critical Thinking
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Johns, Brendan T.; Mewhort, Douglas J. K.; Jones, Michael N. – Cognitive Science, 2019
Distributional models of semantics learn word meanings from contextual co-occurrence patterns across a large sample of natural language. Early models, such as LSA and HAL (Landauer & Dumais, 1997; Lund & Burgess, 1996), counted co-occurrence events; later models, such as BEAGLE (Jones & Mewhort, 2007), replaced counting co-occurrences…
Descriptors: Semantics, Learning Processes, Models, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
de Varda, Andrea Gregor; Strapparava, Carlo – Cognitive Science, 2022
The present paper addresses the study of non-arbitrariness in language within a deep learning framework. We present a set of experiments aimed at assessing the pervasiveness of different forms of non-arbitrary phonological patterns across a set of typologically distant languages. Different sequence-processing neural networks are trained in a set…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Phonology, Language Patterns, Language Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lai, Wei; Rácz, Péter; Roberts, Gareth – Cognitive Science, 2020
How do speakers learn the social meaning of different linguistic variants, and what factors influence how likely a particular social-linguistic association is to be learned? It has been argued that the social meaning of more salient variants should be learned faster, and that learners' pre-existing experience of a variant will influence its…
Descriptors: Language Variation, Second Language Learning, Sociolinguistics, Prior Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stevens, Jon Scott; Gleitman, Lila R.; Trueswell, John C.; Yang, Charles – Cognitive Science, 2017
We evaluate here the performance of four models of cross-situational word learning: two global models, which extract and retain multiple referential alternatives from each word occurrence; and two local models, which extract just a single referent from each occurrence. One of these local models, dubbed "Pursuit," uses an associative…
Descriptors: Semantics, Associative Learning, Probability, Computational Linguistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Arunachalam, Sudha – Cognitive Science, 2017
Children have difficulty comprehending novel verbs in the double object dative (e.g., "Fred blicked the dog a stick") as compared to the prepositional dative (e.g., "Fred blicked a stick to the dog"). We explored this pattern with 3 and 4 year olds (N = 60). In Experiment 1, we replicated the documented difficulty with the…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Language Acquisition, Semantics, Verbs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ouyang, Long; Boroditsky, Lera; Frank, Michael C. – Cognitive Science, 2017
Computational models have shown that purely statistical knowledge about words' linguistic contexts is sufficient to learn many properties of words, including syntactic and semantic category. For example, models can infer that "postman" and "mailman" are semantically similar because they have quantitatively similar patterns of…
Descriptors: Semiotics, Computational Linguistics, Syntax, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Alishahi, Afra; Stevenson, Suzanne – Cognitive Science, 2008
How children go about learning the general regularities that govern language, as well as keeping track of the exceptions to them, remains one of the challenging open questions in the cognitive science of language. Computational modeling is an important methodology in research aimed at addressing this issue. We must determine appropriate learning…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Linguistics, Cognitive Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ramscar, Michael; Yarlett, Daniel – Cognitive Science, 2007
In a series of studies children show increasing mastery of irregular plural forms (such as "mice") simply by producing erroneous over-regularized versions of them (such as "mouses"). We explain this phenomenon in terms of successive approximation in imitation: Children over-regularize early in acquisition because the representations of frequent,…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Morphemes, Linguistics, Feedback (Response)