NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 13 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jacquey, Lisa; Fagard, Jacqueline; Esseily, Rana; O'Regan, J. Kevin – Developmental Psychology, 2020
To benefit from the exploration of their bodies and their physical and social environments, infants need to detect sensorimotor contingencies linking their actions to sensory feedback. This ability, which seems to be present in babies from birth and even in utero, has been widely used by researchers in their study of early development. However, a…
Descriptors: Infants, Psychomotor Skills, Child Development, Sensory Integration
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stormer, Viola S.; Passow, Susanne; Biesenack, Julia; Li, Shu-Chen – Developmental Psychology, 2012
Attention and working memory are fundamental for selecting and maintaining behaviorally relevant information. Not only do both processes closely intertwine at the cognitive level, but they implicate similar functional brain circuitries, namely the frontoparietal and the frontostriatal networks, which are innervated by cholinergic and dopaminergic…
Descriptors: Aging (Individuals), Genetics, Cognitive Development, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bialystok, Ellen – Developmental Psychology, 2010
In 3 experiments, a total of 151 monolingual and bilingual 6-year-old children performed similarly on measures of language and cognitive ability; however, bilinguals solved the global-local and trail-making tasks more rapidly than monolinguals. This bilingual advantage was found not only for the traditionally demanding conditions (incongruent…
Descriptors: Children, Bilingualism, Monolingualism, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ang, Su Yin; Lee, Kerry – Developmental Psychology, 2010
Although visuospatial short-term memory tasks have been found to engage more executive resources than do their phonological counterparts, it remains unclear whether this is due to intrinsic differences between the tasks or differences in participants' experience with them. The authors found 11-year-olds' performances on both visual short-term and…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Children, Spatial Ability, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hund, Alycia M.; Plumert, Jodie M. – Developmental Psychology, 2003
Two experiments examined how information about what objects are influences memory for where objects are located. Seven-, 9-, and 11-year-old children and adults learned the locations of 20 objects marked by dots on the floor of a box. The objects belonged to 4 categories. In one condition, objects belonging to the same category were located in the…
Descriptors: Memory, Children, Adults, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kushnir, Tamar; Gopnik, Alison – Developmental Psychology, 2007
This study examines preschoolers' causal assumptions about spatial contiguity and how these assumptions interact with new evidence in the form of conditional probabilities. Preschoolers saw a toy that activated in the presence of certain objects. Children were shown evidence for the toy's activation rule in the form of patterns of probability: The…
Descriptors: Toys, Inferences, Probability, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hund, Alycia M.; Plumert, Jodie M. – Developmental Psychology, 2007
The authors investigated how 3- and 4-year-old children and adults use relative distance to judge nearbyness. Participants judged whether several blocks were by a landmark. The absolute and relative distance of the blocks from the landmark varied. In Experiment 1, judgments of nearbyness decreased as the distance from the landmark increased, …
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Adults, Geographic Location, Educational Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ehrlich, Stacy B.; Levine, Susan C.; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Developmental Psychology, 2006
On average, men outperform women on mental rotation tasks. Even boys as young as 4 1/2 perform better than girls on simplified spatial transformation tasks. The goal of our study was to explore ways of improving 5-year-olds' performance on a spatial transformation task and to examine the strategies children use to solve this task. We found that…
Descriptors: Gender Differences, Nonverbal Communication, Spatial Ability, Cognitive Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Saxe, Rebecca; Tzelnic, Tania; Carey, Susan – Developmental Psychology, 2007
Preverbal infants can represent the causal structure of events, including distinguishing the agentive and receptive roles and categorizing entities according to stable causal dispositions. This study investigated how infants combine these 2 kinds of causal inference. In Experiments 1 and 2, 9.5-month-olds used the position of a human hand or a…
Descriptors: Toys, Motion, Infant Behavior, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chen, Zhe – Developmental Psychology, 2007
A series of microgenetic experiments was conducted to examine the role of experience on 2.5- to 5-year-old children's discovery of spatial mapping strategies. With experience, 3- to 4-year-olds discovered a strategy for mapping corresponding locations that shared both featural and spatial similarities. When featural and spatial correspondences…
Descriptors: Learning Strategies, Preschool Children, Spatial Ability, Map Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cowan, Nelson; Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe; Kilb, Angela; Saults, J. Scott – Developmental Psychology, 2006
We asked whether the ability to keep in working memory the binding between a visual object and its spatial location changes with development across the life span more than memory for item information. Paired arrays of colored squares were identical or differed in the color of one square, and in the latter case, the changed color was unique on…
Descriptors: Geometric Concepts, Memory, Older Adults, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pelphrey, Kevin A.; Reznick, J. Steven; Goldman, Barbara Davis; Sasson, Noah; Morrow, Judy; Donahoe, Andrea; Hodgson, Katharine – Developmental Psychology, 2004
Eighty 5.5- to 12.5-month-old infants participated in 4 delayed-response procedures challenging shortterm visuospatial memory (STVM), 2 that varied the time between presentation and search and 2 that varied the number of locations. Within each type of challenge, 1 task required a gaze response and 1 required a reach response. There was little…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Infants, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Casasola, Marianella – Developmental Psychology, 2005
Two experiments explored the effect of linguistic input on 18-month-olds' ability to form an abstract categorical representation of support. Infants were habituated to 4 support events (i.e., one object placed on another) and were tested with a novel support and a novel containment event. Infants formed an abstract category of support (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Linguistic Input, Language Acquisition, Spatial Ability