Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 7 |
Descriptor
Source
Developmental Psychology | 18 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 15 |
Reports - Research | 15 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 2 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Grade 7 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Secondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Location
California (Los Angeles) | 1 |
Canada | 1 |
Guatemala | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Italy | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Matching Familiar Figures Test | 3 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Andrew Shtulman; Brandon Goulding; Ori Friedman – Developmental Psychology, 2024
Young children tend to deny the possibility of events that violate their expectations, including events that are merely improbable, like making onion-flavored ice cream or owning a crocodile as a pet. Could this tendency be countered by teaching children more valid strategies for judging possibility? We explored this question by training children…
Descriptors: Children, Thinking Skills, Evaluative Thinking, Age Differences
Cavadel, Elizabeth Woodburn; Frye, Douglas A. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
The current study investigated the role of theory of mind development in school readiness among 120 low-income preschool and kindergarten children. A short-term longitudinal design was used to examine relations among theory of mind, the understanding of teaching, and learning behaviors and their collective role in children's literacy and numeracy…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, School Readiness, Low Income Groups, Preschool Children
Ruggeri, Azzurra; Lombrozo, Tania; Griffiths, Thomas L.; Xu, Fei – Developmental Psychology, 2016
Children are active learners: they learn not only from the information people offer and the evidence they happen to observe, but by actively seeking information. However, children's information search strategies are typically less efficient than those of adults. In two studies, we isolate potential sources of developmental change in how children…
Descriptors: Information Seeking, Search Strategies, Cognitive Style, Cognitive Structures
Furlan, Sarah; Agnoli, Franca; Reyna, Valerie F. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Dual-process theories have been proposed to explain normative and heuristic responses to reasoning and decision-making problems. Standard unitary and dual-process theories predict that normative responses should increase with age. However, research has focused recently on exceptions to this standard pattern, including developmental increases in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Misconceptions, Cognitive Style, Logical Thinking
Weiss, David; Freund, Alexandra M.; Wiese, Bettina S. – Developmental Psychology, 2012
The present research focuses on 2 factors that might help or hurt women to cope with the uncertainties associated with developmental transitions in modern societies (i.e., starting one's first job, graduating from high school, reentry to work after parental leave). We investigate (a) the role of openness to experience in coping with challenging…
Descriptors: Females, Self Efficacy, Longitudinal Studies, Ideology
Galindo, Claudia; Fuller, Bruce – Developmental Psychology, 2010
We know that social competence contributes to young children's adaptation to, and cognitive learning within, classroom settings. Yet initial evidence is mixed on the social competencies that Latino children bring to kindergarten and the extent to which these skills advance cognitive growth. Building from ecocultural and developmental-risk theory,…
Descriptors: School Readiness, Economically Disadvantaged, Young Children, Risk
Correa-Chavez, Maricela; Rogoff, Barbara – Developmental Psychology, 2009
This study investigated differences in attention and learning among Guatemalan Mayan and European American children, ages 5-11 years, who were present but not addressed while their sibling was shown how to construct a novel toy. Each child waited with a distracter toy for her or his turn to make a different toy. Nonaddressed children from Mayan…
Descriptors: Maya (People), Family Involvement, Toys, Children

Hoppe, Christiane M.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1977
Descriptors: Anglo Americans, Cognitive Style, Conflict Resolution, Cross Cultural Studies

Pratt, Michael W.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1993
Reports three studies which (1) investigated children's and adults' concepts of development (COD); (2) observed parents' tutoring their fifth-grade children; and (3) interviewed adults in three age groups about parenting dilemmas. In each study, COD scores were obtained. Results of all studies showed that subjects' stage of COD reasoning was…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Rearing, Children

Smith, J. David; Caplan, Janet – Developmental Psychology, 1988
A cross-age technique was used in two studies to extend understanding of cognitive style development and its cross-cultural generality. Chinese-American children were given the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Results were compared with existing data to determine the style development of children from other cultures. (PCB)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Style, Conceptual Tempo, Cross Cultural Studies

Overton, Willis; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1985
Examined development of conditional reasoning from perspective of competence-moderator-performance approach. Effects of task interpretation and cognitive style as moderator variables for conditional reasoning were examined with 8th, 10th and 12th graders. Results indicated that only 12th graders benefit from training and training generalized to…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Cognitive Style, Competence, Individual Differences

Gjerde, Per F.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1985
In a longitudinal sample of three- through 11-year-olds, age-appropriate versions of the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT) were administered. Uncorrected for attenuation, MFFT error scores were more consistent over time than MFFT latency scores for both girls and boys. Implications of results for validity of MFFT as a measure of cognitive…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Style, Intelligence Quotient, Longitudinal Studies

Ridberg, Eugene H.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1971
Cognitive style was modified in a sample of 50 impulsive and 50 reflective fourth-grade boys. Subjects viewed a film-mediated model displaying a response style opposite to their own cognitive style. The specific cues in the model's behavior which facilitated shifts in cognitive style varied with the intellectual levels of the subject. (Author/WY)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style, Films, Grade 4

Petersen, Anne C. – Developmental Psychology, 1976
This study investigated the relationship of physical manifestations of sex hormone influence to cognitive functioning in a longitudinal sample of 106 adolescent males and 99 females (ages 13, 16, and 18 years). Analyses of the 35 males and 40 females who had all cognitive and physical data at all three ages are reported here. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Style, Elementary Secondary Education, Physical Development

Globerson, Tamar; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1985
Tested whether or not cognitive development (as measured by mental capacity) and cognitive style (as measured by field-dependence/independence) are different dimensions. Results are discussed with regard to Pascual-Leone's model of cognitive development, relevance to stylistic dimension of reflection/impulsivity, and educational implications.…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Style, Elementary Education, Field Dependence Independence
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2