Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 13 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 14 |
Experiments | 14 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Child Development | 5 |
Young Children | 5 |
Adults | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 4 |
Infants | 4 |
Statistical Analysis | 4 |
Children | 3 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 14 |
Author
Barrouillet, Pierre | 2 |
Agnoli, Franca | 1 |
Brackmann, Nathalie | 1 |
Camos, Valerie | 1 |
Cicchino, Jessica B. | 1 |
Denison, Stephanie | 1 |
Friedman, Ori | 1 |
Furlan, Sarah | 1 |
Gaillard, Vinciane | 1 |
Gauffroy, Caroline | 1 |
Gavens, Nathalie | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 13 |
Reports - Research | 13 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 4 |
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 2 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Grade 5 | 2 |
Grade 7 | 2 |
Grade 9 | 2 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
More ▼ |
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Italy | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
Switzerland (Geneva) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Huh, Michelle; Friedman, Ori – Developmental Psychology, 2017
Group ownership is ubiquitous-property is owned by countries, corporations, families, and clubs. However, people cannot understand group ownership by simply relying on their conceptions of ownership by individuals, as group ownership is subject to complexities that do not arise when property is individually owned. We report 6 experiments…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Young Children, Group Dynamics, Ownership
Otgaar, Henry; Howe, Mark L.; Brackmann, Nathalie; van Helvoort, DaniĆ«l H. J. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
We examined whether typical developmental trends in suggestion-induced false memories (i.e., age-related decrease) could be changed. Using theoretical principles from the spontaneous false memory field, we adapted 2 often-used false memory procedures: misinformation (Experiment 1) and memory conformity (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, 7- to…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Adults, Memory
Paulus, Markus – Developmental Psychology, 2016
The ability to act jointly with another person is a fundamental requirement for participation in social life. The current study examines the development of action planning in a joint action context. In 4 experiments, 3-, 5-, and 7-year-old children as well as a group of adults (n = 196) interacted with another person to operate a novel apparatus.…
Descriptors: Social Life, Experiments, Young Children, Adults
Denison, Stephanie; Reed, Christie; Xu, Fei – Developmental Psychology, 2013
How do people make rich inferences from such sparse data? Recent research has explored this inferential ability by investigating probabilistic reasoning in infancy. For example, 8- and 11-month-old infants can make inferences from samples to populations and vice versa (Denison & Xu, 2010a; Xu & Denison, 2009; Xu & Garcia, 2008a). The…
Descriptors: Probability, Infants, Inferences, Young Children
Scott, Jessica C.; Henderson, Annette M. E. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Object labels are valuable communicative tools because their meanings are shared among the members of a particular linguistic community. The current research was conducted to investigate whether 13-month-old infants appreciate that object labels should not be generalized across individuals who have been shown to speak different languages. Using a…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, Experiments, Habituation
Woods, Rebecca J.; Wilcox, Teresa – Developmental Psychology, 2013
A hierarchical progression in infants' ability to use surface features, such as color, as a basis for object individuation in the first year has been well established (Tremoulet, Leslie, & Hall, 2000; Wilcox, 1999). There is evidence, however, that infants' sensitivity to surface features can be increased through multisensory (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Infants, Human Posture, Motor Development, Object Manipulation
Furlan, Sarah; Agnoli, Franca; Reyna, Valerie F. – Developmental Psychology, 2013
Dual-process theories have been proposed to explain normative and heuristic responses to reasoning and decision-making problems. Standard unitary and dual-process theories predict that normative responses should increase with age. However, research has focused recently on exceptions to this standard pattern, including developmental increases in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Misconceptions, Cognitive Style, Logical Thinking
Gauffroy, Caroline; Barrouillet, Pierre – Developmental Psychology, 2011
One of the main tenets of the mental model theory is that when individuals reason, they think about possibilities. According to this theory, reasoning on what is possible from the truth of a sentence would be psychologically basic, whereas reasoning the other way round, on the truth or falsity of a sentence from a given state of affairs, would…
Descriptors: Sentences, Grade 9, Cognitive Development, Child Development
Krist, Horst – Developmental Psychology, 2010
In a series of 3 experiments modeled after infant studies, 3- to- 6-year-old children's intuitive knowledge about support was assessed. Different objects were shown either sufficiently supported or not. Children had to predict whether a block would remain standing on a platform upon release or make perceptual judgments about the possibility of a…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Young Children, Intuition, Physics
Cicchino, Jessica B.; Rakison, David H. – Developmental Psychology, 2008
Three experiments investigated 5- through 8-month-olds' ability to encode self-propelled and caused motion and examined whether processing of motion onset changes when crawling begins. Infants were habituated (Experiments 1 and 2) or familiarized (Experiment 3) with simple causal and noncausal launching events. They then viewed the caused-to-move…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Experiments, Habituation
Nguyen, Simone P. – Developmental Psychology, 2007
Items commonly belong to many categories. Cross-classification is the classification of a single item into more than one category. This research explored 2- to 6-year-old children's use of 2 different category systems for cross-classification: script (e.g., school-time items, birthday party items) and taxonomic (e.g., animals, clothes). The…
Descriptors: Classification, Young Children, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Vinter, Annie; Marot, Valerie – Developmental Psychology, 2007
The authors report on a series of 5 experiments in which 462 5- to 10-year-old children and 109 adults were required to copy geometric figures either with no constraints or following prior exposure to primes consisting of different parsings of the figures. The analysis focused on the graphic strategies adopted by the participants to copy the…
Descriptors: Geometric Concepts, Cognitive Development, Children, Context Effect
Barrouillet, Pierre; Gavens, Nathalie; Vergauwe, Evie; Gaillard, Vinciane; Camos, Valerie – Developmental Psychology, 2009
The time-based resource-sharing model (P. Barrouillet, S. Bernardin, & V. Camos, 2004) assumes that during complex working memory span tasks, attention is frequently and surreptitiously switched from processing to reactivate decaying memory traces before their complete loss. Three experiments involving children from 5 to 14 years of age…
Descriptors: Late Adolescents, Short Term Memory, Children, Experiments

Siegler, Robert S.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1973
Ten- and 11-year-old boys and girls were taught to solve Inhelder and Piaget's pendulum problem and the results of the experiment replicated their expectation that unaided 10- and 11-year-olds do not often solve the pendulum problem. (Editor/RK)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Data Collection, Elementary School Students, Experiments