Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 10 |
Descriptor
Infants | 24 |
Motion | 24 |
Visual Perception | 11 |
Age Differences | 9 |
Infant Behavior | 8 |
Visual Stimuli | 8 |
Perceptual Development | 7 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Developmental Psychology | 4 |
Eye Movements | 4 |
Attention | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 24 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 24 |
Reports - Research | 23 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Audience
Researchers | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Lany, Jill; Thompson, Abbie; Aguero, Ariel – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Words influence cognition well before infants know their meanings. For example, three-month-olds are more likely to form visually based categories when exemplars are paired with spoken words than with sine-wave tones, a likely precursor to learning symbolic relations between words and their referents. However, it is unclear why words have these…
Descriptors: Infants, Naming, Nonverbal Communication, Classification
Kim, Minju; Schachner, Adena – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Dance is a universal human behavior and a crucial component of human musicality. When and how does the motivation and tendency to move to music develop? How does this behavior change as a process of maturation and learning? We characterize infants' earliest dance behavior, leveraging parents' extensive at-home observations of their children.…
Descriptors: Parents, Infants, Dance, Infant Behavior
Gottwald, Janna M.; De Bortoli Vizioli, Aurora; Lindskog, Marcus; Nyström, Pär; L. Ekberg, Therese; von Hofsten, Claes; Gredebäck, Gustaf – Developmental Psychology, 2017
Prospective motor control, a key element of action planning, is the ability to adjust one's actions with respect to task demands and action goals in an anticipatory manner. The current study investigates whether 14-month-olds can prospectively control their reaching actions based on the difficulty of the subsequent action. We used a reach-to-place…
Descriptors: Infants, Psychomotor Skills, Self Control, Difficulty Level
Aktan-Erciyes, Asli; Göksun, Tilbe – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Before infants produce words, they can discriminate changes in motion event components such as manner (how an action is performed) and path (trajectory of an action). Individual differences in nonlinguistic event categorization are related to children's later verb comprehension (Konishi, Stahl, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2016). We asked: (a) Do…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Development, Child Language, Language Acquisition
Xiao, Naiqi G.; Quinn, Paul C.; Liu, Shaoying; Ge, Liezhong; Pascalis, Olivier; Lee, Kang – Developmental Psychology, 2015
Current knowledge about face processing in infancy comes largely from studies using static face stimuli, but faces that infants see in the real world are mostly moving ones. To bridge this gap, 3-, 6-, and 9-month-old Asian infants (N = 118) were familiarized with either moving or static Asian female faces, and then their face recognition was…
Descriptors: Infants, Age Differences, Eye Movements, Motion
Wronski, Caroline; Daum, Moritz M. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Movement perception facilitates spatial orienting of attention in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000). In a series of 4 experiments, we investigated how orienting of attention in infancy is modulated by dynamic stimuli. Experiment 1 (N = 36) demonstrated that 5-month-olds as well as 7-month-olds orient to the direction of a…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Infants, Cues, Attention
Uchiyama, Ichiro; Anderson, David I.; Campos, Joseph J.; Witherington, David; Frankel, Carl B.; Lejeune, Laure; Barbu-Roth, Marianne – Developmental Psychology, 2008
Two studies investigated the role of locomotor experience on visual proprioception in 8-month-old infants. "Visual proprioception" refers to the sense of self-motion induced in a static person by patterns of optic flow. A moving room apparatus permitted displacement of an entire enclosure (except for the floor) or the side walls and…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Visual Perception, Foreign Countries
Cicchino, Jessica B.; Rakison, David H. – Developmental Psychology, 2008
Three experiments investigated 5- through 8-month-olds' ability to encode self-propelled and caused motion and examined whether processing of motion onset changes when crawling begins. Infants were habituated (Experiments 1 and 2) or familiarized (Experiment 3) with simple causal and noncausal launching events. They then viewed the caused-to-move…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Experiments, Habituation
Johnson, Scott P.; Davidow, Juliet; Hall-Haro, Cynthia; Frank, Michael C. – Developmental Psychology, 2008
Adults have little difficulty perceiving objects as complete despite occlusion, but newborn infants perceive moving partly occluded objects solely in terms of visible surfaces. The developmental mechanisms leading to perceptual completion have never been adequately explained. Here, the authors examine the potential contributions of oculomotor…
Descriptors: Infants, Visual Perception, Cognitive Development, Motion

Bahrick, Lorraine E.; Watson, John S. – Developmental Psychology, 1985
In three experiments, 5-month-old infants discriminated between a perfectly contingent live display of their own leg motion and a noncontingent display of self or a peer. They showed this discrimination by preferential fixation of the noncontingent display. (Author/NH)
Descriptors: Body Image, Infants, Motion, Visual Stimuli

Aslin, Richard N.; Shea, Sandra L. – Developmental Psychology, 1990
Results provided evidence that 6- and 12-week-old infants' preferences for a moving set of stripes over a stationary set were based on the velocity rather than on the temporal frequency of stripe movement. Estimated velocity thresholds of 9 degrees per second for 6 week olds and 4 degrees per second for 12 week olds extended results of previous…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Infants, Motion, Perception

Finlay, David; Ivinskis, Algis – Developmental Psychology, 1984
Compares the responses of four-month-olds in two situations, one in which a moving peripheral stimulus follows a central fixation stimulus and another where the peripheral stimulus is simultaneous with the central fixation. Simultaneous presentation decreases visual orientation to the peripheral stimulus, but cardiac data indicate that the…
Descriptors: Heart Rate, Infants, Motion, Perception

Werker, Janet F.; Cohen, Leslie B.; Lloyd, Valerie L.; Stager, Christine.; Casasola, Marianella – Developmental Psychology, 1998
In six experiments, infants were habituated to word-object pairings and then presented with a familiar word and object in a previously seen and a new pairing. Found that 14-month olds formed word-object associations under these conditions when the objects were moving; 8- to 12-month olds did not form associations but appeared to process the word…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Infants, Language Acquisition, Motion
Rakison, David H. – Developmental Psychology, 2006
In 3 experiments, the author investigated 16- to 20-month-old infants' attention to dynamic and static parts in learning about self-propelled objects. In Experiment 1, infants were habituated to simple noncausal events in which a geometric figure with a single moving part started to move without physical contact from an identical geometric figure…
Descriptors: Infants, Experiments, Visual Learning, Geometric Concepts

Gibson, Eleanor J.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1978
Five-month-old infants were habituated to three types of visually presented rigid motion, with duration of fixation as the dependent measure. After reaching a criterion of habituation, a fourth rigid motion (not habituated) and a deformation were presented. (Author/SS)
Descriptors: Eye Fixations, Infant Behavior, Infants, Motion
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2