Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 6 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Predictor Variables | 10 |
Vocabulary Development | 10 |
Language Skills | 4 |
Mothers | 4 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Language Acquisition | 3 |
Longitudinal Studies | 3 |
Toddlers | 3 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Gender Differences | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 10 |
Author
Eisenberg, Nancy | 2 |
Spinrad, Tracy L. | 2 |
Bell, Martha Ann | 1 |
Boom, Jan | 1 |
Brink, Kimberly A. | 1 |
Bruce, Madeleine | 1 |
Cheung, Him | 1 |
Cunningham, Anne E. | 1 |
Demir, Özlem Ece | 1 |
Duncan, Robert J. | 1 |
Edwards, Alison | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 10 |
Reports - Research | 10 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 3 |
Clinical Evaluation of… | 1 |
MacArthur Communicative… | 1 |
Wechsler Preschool and… | 1 |
Woodcock Johnson Psycho… | 1 |
Woodcock Johnson Tests of… | 1 |
Woodcock Reading Mastery Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Bruce, Madeleine; Miyazaki, Yasuo; Bell, Martha Ann – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Receptive vocabulary development was examined in 313 children (151 girls; 78% White) as a function of infant attention and maternal education (66% of mothers held a college degree or higher). Attention was measured at 10 months using a dynamic puppet task and receptive vocabulary was measured at 3-, 4-, 6-, and 9 years of age using the Peabody…
Descriptors: Receptive Language, Vocabulary Development, Infants, Attention
Brink, Kimberly A.; Wellman, Henry M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Children acquire extensive knowledge from others. Today, children receive information from not only people but also technological devices, like social robots. Two studies assessed whether young children appropriately trust technological informants. One hundred and four 3-year-olds learned the names of novel objects from either a pair of social…
Descriptors: Robotics, Trust (Psychology), Toddlers, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Duncan, Robert J.; Schmitt, Sara A.; Vandell, Deborah Lowe – Developmental Psychology, 2019
This study examines associations between stimulating-responsive social interactions with mothers and nonparental childcare providers during the first 3 years of life and children's vocabulary and mathematics skills through age 15 (N = 1,364). Additive relations were found in which more stimulating-responsive interactions with mothers and with…
Descriptors: Mothers, Parent Child Relationship, Toddlers, Infants
Lecheile, Bridget M.; Spinrad, Tracy L.; Xu, Xiaoye; Lopez, Jamie; Eisenberg, Nancy – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Previous research has shown that home environment plays an important role in children's early language skills. Yet, few researchers have examined the unique role of family-level factors (socioeconomic status [SES], household chaos) on children's learning or focused on the longitudinal processes that might explain their relations to children's…
Descriptors: Family Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Language Skills, Language Acquisition
Verhagen, Josje; Boom, Jan; Mulder, Hanna; de Bree, Elise; Leseman, Paul – Developmental Psychology, 2019
The aim of this longitudinal study is to evaluate 3 views on the relationship between nonword repetition and vocabulary: (i) the storage-based view that considers nonword repetition, a measure of phonological storage, as the driving force behind vocabulary development, (ii) the lexical restructuring view that considers improvements in nonword…
Descriptors: Correlation, Word Recognition, Repetition, Vocabulary
Ouellette, Gene; Sénéchal, Monique – Developmental Psychology, 2017
In this study we evaluated whether the sophistication of children's invented spellings in kindergarten was predictive of subsequent reading and spelling in Grade 1, while also considering the influence of well-known precursors. Children in their first year of schooling (mean age = 66 months; N = 171) were assessed on measures of oral vocabulary,…
Descriptors: Invented Spelling, Kindergarten, Predictor Variables, Reading Ability
Demir, Özlem Ece; Rowe, Meredith L.; Heller, Gabriella; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C. – Developmental Psychology, 2015
This study examines the role of a particular kind of linguistic input--talk about the past and future, pretend, and explanations, that is, talk that is decontextualized--in the development of vocabulary, syntax, and narrative skill in typically developing (TD) children and children with pre- or perinatal brain injury (BI). Decontextualized talk…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Syntax, Language Skills, Children
Eisenberg, Nancy; Vidmar, Masa; Spinrad, Tracy L.; Eggum, Natalie D.; Edwards, Alison; Gaertner, Bridget; Kupfer, Anne – Developmental Psychology, 2010
Findings on the relation of maternal verbal teaching strategies to children's effortful control (EC; i.e., self-regulation) are limited in quantity and somewhat inconsistent. In this study, children's EC was assessed at 18, 30, and 42 months (ns = 255, 229, and 209, respectively) with adults' reports and a behavioral measure. Mothers' verbal…
Descriptors: Mothers, Emotional Development, Teaching Methods, Verbal Communication

Cheung, Him – Developmental Psychology, 1996
Nonword span (nonwords conforming to English phonology) was used to measure phonological memory and was found to be generally predictive of the number of trials needed for acquiring certain English (second-language) new words for 84 Hong Kong seventh graders. This relationship, however, was preserved only in those students whose English vocabulary…
Descriptors: Elementary Secondary Education, English (Second Language), Foreign Countries, Junior High School Students

Cunningham, Anne E.; Stanovich, Keith E. – Developmental Psychology, 1997
Studied reading comprehension, vocabulary, general knowledge, and print exposure of 11th graders who completed reading battery 10 years earlier. Found that first-grade reading ability predicted all 11th-grade outcomes--even when cognitive ability was partialed out--and was linked to print exposure, even after 11th-grade reading comprehension…
Descriptors: Beginning Reading, Cognitive Ability, Cultural Literacy, Elementary School Students