Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Child Development | 2 |
Imitation | 2 |
Perceptual Motor Learning | 2 |
Age | 1 |
Discovery Learning | 1 |
Evidence | 1 |
Experiential Learning | 1 |
Infant Behavior | 1 |
Learning Processes | 1 |
Modeling (Psychology) | 1 |
Neonates | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Science | 2 |
Author
Barr, Rachel | 1 |
Heyes, Cecilia | 1 |
Patterson, Eric M. | 1 |
Ray, Elizabeth | 1 |
Renner, Elizabeth | 1 |
Schilder, Brian | 1 |
Subiaul, Francys | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Reports - Research | 1 |
Education Level
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Subiaul, Francys; Patterson, Eric M.; Schilder, Brian; Renner, Elizabeth; Barr, Rachel – Developmental Science, 2015
In contrast to other primates, human children's imitation performance goes from low to high fidelity soon after infancy. Are such changes associated with the development of other forms of learning? We addressed this question by testing 215 children (26-59 months) on two social conditions (imitation, emulation)--involving a demonstration--and two…
Descriptors: Young Children, Child Development, Imitation, Learning Processes
Ray, Elizabeth; Heyes, Cecilia – Developmental Science, 2011
Imitation requires the imitator to solve the correspondence problem--to translate visual information from modelled action into matching motor output. It has been widely accepted for some 30 years that the correspondence problem is solved by a specialized, innate cognitive mechanism. This is the conclusion of a poverty of the stimulus argument,…
Descriptors: Neonates, Imitation, Visual Stimuli, Perceptual Motor Learning