Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 6 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 11 |
Descriptor
Discourse Analysis | 11 |
Pragmatics | 11 |
Interpersonal Communication | 5 |
Language Usage | 5 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Computational Linguistics | 3 |
Intention | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 2 |
Criticism | 2 |
Cues | 2 |
Figurative Language | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Discourse Processes: A… | 11 |
Author
Trott, Sean | 2 |
Bergen, Benjamin | 1 |
Boylan, James | 1 |
Chen, Qingrong | 1 |
Ferretti, Todd R. | 1 |
Harwood, Jenna | 1 |
Katz, Albert N. | 1 |
Kovaz, David | 1 |
Krekoski, Ross | 1 |
Kreuz, Roger J. | 1 |
M. Meghan Davidson | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 11 |
Reports - Research | 11 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Yuki Arita – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2024
This conversation analytic study offers an empirical analysis of the Japanese turn-initial interjection "are." The interjectional "are" is said to be pragmatized from its use as a distal demonstrative and has been considered as an expression of a speaker's internal state of being surprised at something. In contrast, this study…
Descriptors: Discourse Analysis, Language Usage, Japanese, Interpersonal Communication
Trott, Sean; Rossano, Federico – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2020
Requesting plays a key role in human communication. One can request the same thing in multiple ways (e.g., "Pass the salt" vs. "Could you pass the salt?"). How do speakers determine which request form to produce? And how does this choice affect a recipient's evaluation of a request? Previous analyses of naturalistic…
Descriptors: Interpersonal Communication, Communication Skills, Speech Communication, Discourse Analysis
M. Meghan Davidson – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2023
Text social information includes the cognitive processes and social communication skills that support real or hypothetical human thought or interaction. The current measure of text social information is genre. However, genre is a limited measure because of poor operationalization, limited specificity, and overlap with structural and linguistic…
Descriptors: Picture Books, Information Sources, Predictive Validity, Cognitive Processes
Trott, Sean; Bergen, Benjamin – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2019
People often speak ambiguously, as in the case of "indirect requests." Certain indirect requests are conventional and thus straightforward to interpret, such as "Can you turn on the heater?", but others require substantial additional inference, such as "It's cold in here." How do comprehenders make inferences about a…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Speech Acts, Discourse Analysis, Intention
Mauchand, Maël; Vergis, Nikos; Pell, Marc D. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2020
In spoken discourse, understanding irony requires the apprehension of subtle cues, such as the speaker's tone of voice (prosody), which often reveal the speaker's affective stance toward the listener in the context of the utterance. To shed light on the interplay of linguistic content and prosody on impressions of spoken criticisms and compliments…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Intonation, Suprasegmentals, Cues
Xu, Xiaodong; Chen, Qingrong; Panther, Klaus-Uwe; Wu, Yicheng – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2018
This study investigates the influence of causal and concessive relations on discourse coherence in Chinese by means of eye movement and self-paced reading techniques. We use the sentential structure like "NP[subscript HUMAN] moved from place A to place B, {because ([Chinese characters omitted] yinwei) /although ([Chinese characters omitted]…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Pacing, Reading Instruction, Comparative Analysis
Boylan, James; Katz, Albert N. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2013
In the context of texts that depicted either a minimally confrontational conversation (study 1) or a more confrontational argument (study 2) with a close friend, the use of ironic criticism was rated as being more humorous, polite, and positive, yet also as more sarcastic and mocking than direct criticism. Although our results were consistent with…
Descriptors: Criticism, Figurative Language, Persuasive Discourse, Humor
Krekoski, Ross – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2012
Recent studies illustrate cases of turn continuations that are not necessarily criterially dependent on clausal syntax (Couper-Kuhlen & Ono, 2007; Ford, Fox, & Thompson, 2002), advancing a more multidimensional construal of turn expansions, in general, which, as Auer (2007) put it, "is not a syntactic issue alone" (p. 651). This study further…
Descriptors: Japanese, Phrase Structure, Syntax, Pragmatics
Ferretti, Todd R.; Singer, Murray; Harwood, Jenna – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2013
We used ERP methodology to investigate how readers validate discourse concepts and update situation models when those concepts followed factive (e.g., knew) and nonfactive (e.g., "guessed") verbs, and also when they were true, false, or indeterminate with reference to previous discourse. Following factive verbs, early (P2) and later brain…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Diagnostic Tests, Linguistic Theory, Verbs
Kovaz, David; Kreuz, Roger J.; Riordan, Monica A. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2013
Sarcasm production and comprehension have been traditionally described in terms of pragmatic factors. Lexical cues have received less attention, but they may be important potential indicators. A major obstacle to examining such features is determining sarcastic intent. One solution is to analyze statements explicitly marked as being sarcastic.…
Descriptors: Language Usage, Electronic Publishing, Phrase Structure, Negative Attitudes
Noice, Helga; Noice, Tony – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2007
A large body of research has shown that verbal phrases such as "move the pen" are better remembered when they are physically enacted than when the same phrases are studied under standard verbal learning instructions (e.g., Engelkamp & Krumnacker, 1980). More recently, a non-literal enactment effect was discovered in which verbal material that was…
Descriptors: Verbal Learning, Pragmatics, Measurement Techniques, Motion