Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 6 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 6 |
Toys | 6 |
Infants | 5 |
Play | 4 |
Eye Movements | 3 |
Infant Behavior | 3 |
Child Development | 2 |
Responses | 2 |
Toddlers | 2 |
Affective Behavior | 1 |
Aggression | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Infancy | 6 |
Author
Chadwick, Andrea | 1 |
Collicott, Cherie | 1 |
Collins, Stephanie | 1 |
Courage, Mary L. | 1 |
Degnan, Kathryn Amey | 1 |
Fox, Nathan A. | 1 |
Frank, Michael C. | 1 |
Hane, Amie Ashley | 1 |
Hay, Dale F. | 1 |
He, Jie | 1 |
Henderson, Heather A. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (London) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hay, Dale F.; Hurst, Sarah-Louise; Waters, Cerith S.; Chadwick, Andrea – Infancy, 2011
The two aims of the study were (a) to determine when infants begin to use force intentionally to defend objects to which they might have a claim and (b) to examine the relationship between toddlers' instrumental use of force and their tendencies to make possession claims. Infants' and toddlers' reactions to peers' attempts to take their toys were…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Toys, Ownership
Frank, Michael C.; Vul, Edward; Saxe, Rebecca – Infancy, 2012
How do young children direct their attention to other people in the natural world? Although many studies have examined the perception of faces and of goal-directed actions, relatively little work has focused on what children will look at in complex and unconstrained viewing environments. To address this question, we showed videos of objects,…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Measurement Techniques, Infant Behavior, Attention
Setliff, Alissa E.; Courage, Mary L. – Infancy, 2011
The effect of background television on 6- and 12-month-olds' attention during 20 min of toy play was examined. During the first or second half of the session, a clip from a variety of commonly available television programs was presented. The duration and frequency of infants' looks to the toys and to the television indicated that regardless of age…
Descriptors: Television Viewing, Play, Infants, Toys
He, Jie; Degnan, Kathryn Amey; McDermott, Jennifer Martin; Henderson, Heather A.; Hane, Amie Ashley; Xu, Qinmei; Fox, Nathan A. – Infancy, 2010
The relations among infant anger reactivity, approach behavior, and frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry, and their relations to inhibitory control and behavior problems in early childhood were examined within the context of a longitudinal study of temperament. Two hundred nine infants' anger expressions to arm restraint were observed at 4…
Descriptors: Behavior Problems, Infants, Inhibition, Psychological Patterns
Marsh, Heidi L.; Stavropoulos, Jennifer; Nienhuis, Tom; Legerstee, Maria – Infancy, 2010
Behne, Carpenter, Call, and Tomasello (2005) showed that 9- to 18-month-olds, but not 6-month-olds, differentiated between people who were unwilling and unable to share toys. As the outcome of the two tasks is the same (i.e., the toy is not shared), the infants must respond to the different goals of the actor. However, visual habituation paradigms…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Toys, Age Differences
Collicott, Cherie; Collins, Stephanie; Moore, Chris – Infancy, 2009
Infants follow the gaze of an individual with whom they are directly interacting by the end of the first year. By 18 months infants are capable of learning novel words in observational (or third-party) contexts (Floor & Akhtar, 2006). To examine third-party gaze following in 12- and 18-month-olds, the parent and experimenter engaged in a…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Infants, Eye Movements, Vocabulary Development