NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 19 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Klein, Christoph; Arend, Isabel C.; Beauducel, Andre; Shapiro, Kimron L. – Intelligence, 2011
The failure to correctly report two targets ("T[subscript 1]", "T[subscript 2]") that follow each other in close temporal proximity has been called the "attentional blink" (AB). The AB has, so far, mainly been studied using experimental approaches. The present studies investigated individual differences in AB performance, revealing (among further…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Individual Differences, Short Term Memory, Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Arendasy, Martin E.; Sommer, Markus – Intelligence, 2010
In complex three-dimensional mental rotation tasks males have been reported to score up to one standard deviation higher than females. However, this effect size estimate could be compromised by the presence of gender bias at the item level, which calls the validity of purely quantitative performance comparisons into question. We hypothesized that…
Descriptors: Effect Size, Psychometrics, Gender Differences, Gender Bias
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schroeders, Ulrich; Wilhelm, Oliver; Bucholtz, Nina – Intelligence, 2010
Receptive foreign language proficiency is usually measured with reading and listening comprehension tasks. A novel approach to assess such proficiencies--viewing comprehension--is based on the presentation of short instructional videos followed by one or more comprehension questions concerning the preceding video stimulus. In order to evaluate a…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Listening Comprehension, Intelligence, Psychometrics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Reeve, Charlie L.; Bonaccio, Silvia – Intelligence, 2008
Although test anxiety is typically negatively related to performance on cognitive ability tests, little research has systematically investigated whether differences in test anxiety result in measurement bias on cognitive ability tests. The current paper uses a structural equation modeling technique to explicitly test for measurement bias due to…
Descriptors: Structural Equation Models, Test Anxiety, Cognitive Ability, Bias
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vock, Miriam; Holling, Heinz – Intelligence, 2008
The objective of this study is to explore the potential for developing IRT-based working memory scales for assessing specific working memory components in children (8-13 years). These working memory scales should measure cognitive abilities reliably in the upper range of ability distribution as well as in the normal range, and provide a…
Descriptors: Test Items, Academic Achievement, Factor Structure, Factor Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vigneau, Francois; Bors, Douglas A. – Intelligence, 2008
Various taxonomies of Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) items have been proposed in the literature to account for performance on the test. In the present article, three such taxonomies based on information processing, namely Carpenter, Just and Shell's [Carpenter, P.A., Just, M.A., & Shell, P., (1990). What one intelligence test…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Intelligence Tests, Factor Analysis, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Watkins, Marley W.; Lei, Pui-Wa; Canivez, Gary L. – Intelligence, 2007
There has been considerable debate regarding the causal precedence of intelligence and academic achievement. Some researchers view intelligence and achievement as identical constructs. Others believe that the relationship between intelligence and achievement is reciprocal. Still others assert that intelligence is causally related to achievement.…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Academic Achievement, Achievement Tests, Scores
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rammsayer, Thomas H.; Brandler, Susanne – Intelligence, 2007
The relation between general intelligence (psychometric "g") and temporal resolution capacity of the central nervous system was examined by assessing performance on eight different temporal tasks in a sample of 100 participants. Correlational and principal component analyses suggested a unitary timing mechanism, referred to as temporal "g".…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Reaction Time, Multiple Regression Analysis, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deary, Ian J.; Strand, Steve; Smith, Pauline; Fernandes, Cres – Intelligence, 2007
This 5-year prospective longitudinal study of 70,000+ English children examined the association between psychometric intelligence at age 11 years and educational achievement in national examinations in 25 academic subjects at age 16. The correlation between a latent intelligence trait (Spearman's "g"from CAT2E) and a latent trait of…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Intelligence, Longitudinal Studies, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smith, Glen A.; Stanley, Gordon – Intelligence, 1987
By comparing the profile of "g" loadings for a set of psychometric test scores, it is shown that general intelligence is related to timed performance measures from perceptual-motor tasks, more strongly on more complex tasks. The profile of the "g" loadings was predictable from the response-time (RT) psychometric test time correlations for four RT…
Descriptors: Intelligence, Perceptual Motor Coordination, Psychometrics, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Jensen, Arthur R. – Intelligence, 1997
Data from a previous adoption study using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised show that the genetic effect is reflected by psychometric "g" (general intelligence) to a greater degree than is the environmental effect. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that "g" largely reflects the genetic component…
Descriptors: Adoption, Biological Influences, Genetics, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Luo, Dasen; Thompson, Lee A.; Detterman, Douglas K. – Intelligence, 2006
The present study evaluated the criterion validity of the aggregated tasks of basic cognitive processes (TBCP). In age groups from 6 to 19 of the Woodcock-Johnson III Cognitive Abilities and Achievement Tests normative sample, the aggregated TBCP, i.e., the processing speed and working memory clusters, correlate with measures of scholastic…
Descriptors: Psychometrics, Structural Equation Models, Predictive Validity, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Thorndike, Robert L. – Intelligence, 1994
Evidence is presented in support of psychometric "g," arguing that it accounts for most of the prediction of educational and vocational criteria that is possible from a battery of tests of cognitive tasks. Providing the best measure of "g" is explored. (SLD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Criteria, Evaluation Methods, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Jensen, Arthur R.; Weng, Li-Jen – Intelligence, 1994
The stability of psychometric "g," the general factor of intelligence, is investigated in simulated correlation matrices and in typical empirical data from a large battery of mental tests. "G" is robust and almost invariant across methods of analysis. A reasonable strategy for estimating "g" is suggested. (SLD)
Descriptors: Correlation, Estimation (Mathematics), Factor Analysis, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carroll, John B. – Intelligence, 1991
Because they used an inappropriate statistical procedure, J. H. Kranzler and A. R. Jensen (1991) have not demonstrated that a factor of general intelligence ("g") depends on several independent factors. A factorial reanalysis of their data suggests that speed and efficiency of information processing are important in "g." (SLD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Measurement, College Students, Factor Analysis, Factor Structure
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2