Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Source
Journal of Child Language | 19 |
Author
Baldwin, Dare A. | 1 |
Bobb, Susan C. | 1 |
Casillas, Marisa | 1 |
Chapman, Robin S. | 1 |
Charney, Rosalind | 1 |
Clark, Eve V. | 1 |
De Cat, Cécile | 1 |
De Villiers, Jill G. | 1 |
Desai, Sneha S. | 1 |
Erb, Melinda | 1 |
Farrar, Michael Jeffrey | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 14 |
Reports - Research | 12 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Hungary | 1 |
Philippines | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Clinical Evaluation of… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
De Cat, Cécile; Melia, Tara – Journal of Child Language, 2022
The Sentence Structure sub-test (SST) of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF) aims to "measure the acquisition of grammatical (structural) rules at the sentence level". Although originally designed for clinical practice with monolingual children, components of the CELF, such as the SST, are often used to inform…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Language Tests, Reading Comprehension, Cognitive Processes
Casillas, Marisa; Bobb, Susan C.; Clark, Eve V. – Journal of Child Language, 2016
Young children answer questions with longer delays than adults do, and they do not reach typical adult response times until several years later. We hypothesized that this prolonged pattern of delay in children's timing results from competing demands: to give an answer, children must understand a question while simultaneously planning and…
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Caregiver Child Relationship, Interpersonal Communication
Lukacs, Agnes; Pleh, Csaba; Racsmany, Mihaly – Journal of Child Language, 2007
We present data on the language of space in Hungarian individuals with Williams syndrome (WS; 19 in the first, 15 in the second study, between 8;0 and 21;11) and a verbal control (VC) group of typically developing (TD; 19 in the first, 15 in the second study, between 3;5 and 10;7) children from: (1) a study of elicited production and comprehension…
Descriptors: Mental Retardation, Spatial Ability, Interaction, Foreign Countries

Tomasello, Michael; Farrar, Michael Jeffrey – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Describes a lexical training program developed to teach object, visible movement, and invisible movement words to children at stage 5 (N=7) and stage 6 (N=16) object permanence development. Stage 6 children learned all three types of words equally well, while stage 5 children learned object and visible movement but not invisible movement words.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Structures, Comprehension

Charney, Rosalind – Journal of Child Language, 1979
Reports on an experiment, performed on seven children, designed to show that children understand "here" and "there" with the self as reference point before they understand words such as these with reference to other speakers as reference points. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Egocentrism

Chapman, Robin S.; Thompson, Jean – Journal of Child Language, 1980
Previous research has reported instances in which some two-year-olds failed to overextend in comprehension what they overextended in use. Fremgen and Fay found no instance of overextension in comprehension in separate experiments. From this Fremgen and Fay conclude children never overextend in comprehension. This conclusion is re-evaluated here.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Style, Comprehension

Gibbs, Raymond W. – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Examination of the effect of two linguistic factors on kindergarten through fourth-grade students' understanding of idioms indicated that the younger subjects better understood syntactically frozen idioms than those presented in various syntactic forms, while older subjects comprehended both kinds. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Children, Cognitive Processes

Townsend, David J.; Erb, Melinda – Journal of Child Language, 1975
In an experiment in which preschool children were asked questions such as "Which box is taller than it is fat?" the results were interpreted to mean that the linguistic strategy of attending to the first clause is more resistant to change than the preference for simply choosing the largest object. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Language Patterns
Saylor, Megan M.; Baldwin, Dare A. – Journal of Child Language, 2004
The ability to understand references to the absent enables conversation to move beyond the here-and-now to matters distant in both space and time. Such understanding requires appreciating the relation between language and communicative intent: one must recognize speakers' intentions to use language to converge on a shared conversational focus that…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Caregivers, Infants, Language Acquisition

Stevenson, Rosemary J.; Pollitt, Caroline – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Investigation of two- to four-year-olds' (N=20) understanding of temporal terms indicated that children were more likely to understand sentences using simple tasks, materials, and commands than more complicated sentences used in previous research. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Adverbs, Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension
Mazzocco, Michelle M. M.; Myers, Gwen F.; Thompson, Laurie A.; Desai, Sneha S. – Journal of Child Language, 2003
This study was designed to examine factors associated with literal interpretations of homonyms. Participants were 212 second graders, ages 7;0-8;11, who listened to a story containing 16 key words. The key words were homonymous words ("pseudo-homonyms"), nonsense words, or familiar words used accurately. While listening to the story, children…
Descriptors: Grade 2, Word Recognition, Visual Stimuli, Story Telling

Horgan, Dianne – Journal of Child Language, 1978
How a child answers questions provides information about how he or she processes input. A child's early responses to questions at age one year, three months, were compared to her responses at one year, seven months, when she was in the two-word stage. (SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Discourse Analysis

Sachs, Jacqueline; Truswell, Lynn – Journal of Child Language, 1978
Twelve one-word-stage children were given minimally contrasting two-word instructions. Since non-linguistic cues were eliminated, comprehension involved making non-syntactic inferences from the word combinations. The children could respond correctly to some of the instructions, and even carried out some unfamiliar activities. (Author/SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension

De Villiers, Jill G.; Flusberg, Helen B. Tager – Journal of Child Language, 1975
Children aged 2 - 4 were tested to determine the effects of plausibility on comprehension of negative statements. It was found that negatives about an exceptional item in an array, i.e. plausible negatives, were understood before implausible negatives. Other results are described and discussed. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Language Patterns

Kavanaugh, Robert D. – Journal of Child Language, 1979
Sentences were constructed in which the terms "before" and "after" were embedded in logically constrained and logically reversible sequences. The preschool children in the study found the constrained sentences easier to comprehend. (Author/CFM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Language Acquisition
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2