NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 24 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Franchin, Laura; Savazzi, Federica; Neira-Gutierrez, Isabel Cristina; Surian, Luca – Journal of Child Language, 2019
Infants begin to understand some of the meanings of the adjective "good" at around thirteen months, but it is not clear when they start to map it to concepts in the moral domain. We investigated infants' and toddlers' knowledge of "good" in the domains of help and fairness. Participants at 20 and 30 months were shown computer…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Language, Cognitive Mapping, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Emberson, Lauren L.; Loncar, Nicole; Mazzei, Carolyn; Treves, Isaac – Journal of Child Language, 2019
Learners preferentially interpret novel nouns at the basic level ('dog') rather than at a more narrow level ('Labrador'). This 'basic-level bias' is mitigated by statistics: children and adults are more likely to interpret a novel noun at a more narrow label if they witness 'a suspicious coincidence' -- the word applied to three exemplars of the…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Nouns, Language Processing, Inferences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hao, Meiling; Liu, Youyi; Shu, Hua; Xing, Ailing; Jiang, Ying; Li, Ping – Journal of Child Language, 2015
In this paper we report a large-scale developmental study of early productive vocabulary acquisition by 928 Chinese-speaking children aged between 1;0 and 2;6, using the Early Vocabulary Inventory for Mandarin Chinese (Hao, Shu, Xing & Li, 2008). The results show that: (i) social words, especially words for people, are the predominant type of…
Descriptors: Mandarin Chinese, Developmental Stages, Child Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Koring, Loes; de Mulder, Hannah – Journal of Child Language, 2015
This paper investigates six- to nine-year-old children's acquisition of evidentiality. In two minimally different tasks we assess whether children can be made to use a particular source of information by presenting them with a specific evidential term. That is, we assess whether children have an explicit awareness of the source requirement of the…
Descriptors: Information Sources, Evidence, Young Children, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ferguson, Brock; Waxman, Sandra – Journal of Child Language, 2017
Language exerts a powerful influence on our concepts. We review evidence documenting the developmental origins of a precocious link between language and object categories in very young infants. This collection of studies documents a cascading process in which early links between language and cognition provide the foundation for later, more precise…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Concept Formation, Classification, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stites, Lauren J.; Ozcaliskan, Seyda – Journal of Child Language, 2013
Time is frequently expressed with spatial motion, using one of three different metaphor types: moving-time, moving-ego, and sequence-as-position. Previous work shows that children can understand and explain moving-time metaphors by age five (Ozcaliskan, 2005). In this study, we focus on all three metaphor types for time, and ask whether metaphor…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Child Development, Developmental Stages, Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Krott, Andrea; Gagne, Christina L.; Nicoladis, Elena – Journal of Child Language, 2010
The present study investigates children's bias when interpreting novel noun-noun compounds (e.g. "kig donka") that refer to combinations of novel objects (kig and donka). More specifically, it investigates children's understanding of modifier-head relations of the compounds and their preference for HAS or LOCATED relations (e.g. a donka that HAS a…
Descriptors: Nouns, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Child Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Andersen, Elaine S. – Journal of Child Language, 1975
Children aged 3, 6, 9 and 12 years were asked to name and sort 25 different drinking vessels. Results showed three stages: (1) they ove rextend the term "cup"; (2) they focus only on certain perceptual properties; (3) they show growing awareness of functional properties and hence the vagueness of the boundary. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Definitions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lucariello, Joan – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Examination of object word learning and use in beginning (vocabulary of less than 50 words) and advanced (vocabulary of more than 50 words) infant speakers indicated that both groups formed concepts, learned, and generalized words for the to-be-learned objects. Advanced speakers learned more words and concepts and engaged in broader generalization…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Dore, John; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1976
Two transitional phases in the child's early language development are described; the first occurs between prelinguistic vocalization and one-word speech and the second between one-word and patterned speech. Cognitive, linguistic and affective inputs to the acquisition of reference and syntax are discussed in the light of the transitional…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Andrich, Gail Rex; Tager-Flusberg, Helen – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Reports two studies which investigated the acquisition of color terms by preschool children. The first was designed to clarify the role of certain conceptual factors in the acquisition of color terms. The second explored how input may interact with these conceptual factors and help to guide the acquisition of color words. (SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Color, Comprehension, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
French, Lucia A.; Brown, Ann L. – Journal of Child Language, 1977
Preschool children were required to act out a series of two-event sequences conjoined by either "before" or "after." Performance was markedly superior for meaningfully ordered sequences than for arbitrarily ordered sequences. It is suggested that the meanings of "before" and "after" must be acquired in situations which provide contextual support.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moore, Chris; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1995
Examines the development of children's understanding of the difference between "want" and "need" in two different experiments. The first experiment required the children to respond verbally in choosing between the two concepts; the second required them to give an object to one of two characters who had made a request using…
Descriptors: Analysis of Variance, Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Watson, Rita – Journal of Child Language, 1985
Presents a brief theoretical statement on definition and then discusses a study of the development of definition in children aged 5 to 10. The development of definition is characterized as the gradual articulation of a conventional definitional form out of more general forms of ordinary oral discourse. (NCR)
Descriptors: Child Language, Children, Concept Formation, Definitions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Roberts, Kenneth; Horowitz, Frances Degen – Journal of Child Language, 1986
In three experiments, a multiple habituation paradigm was used to examine the ability of 7- and 9-month-old prelinguistic infants to form a natural, basic-level object category. Findings constitute independent evidence for the existence of a linguistically relevant nonlinguistic category prior to the onset of word comprehension. (SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2