NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 179 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fu, Genyue; Evans, Angela D.; Xu, Fen; Lee, Kang – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This study investigated whether young children make strategic decisions about whether to lie to conceal a transgression based on the lie recipient's knowledge. In Experiment 1, 168 3- to 5-year-olds were asked not to peek at the toy in the experimenter's absence, and the majority of children peeked. Children were questioned about their…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Strategic Planning, Experiments, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mondloch, Catherine J.; Horner, Matthew; Mian, Jasmine – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Adults' and 8-year-old children's perception of emotional faces is disrupted when faces are presented in the context of incongruent body postures (e.g., when a sad face is displayed on a fearful body) if the two emotions are highly similar (e.g., sad/fear) but not if they are highly dissimilar (e.g., sad/happy). The current research investigated…
Descriptors: Fear, Cognitive Development, Human Posture, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brown, Helen; Weighall, Anna; Henderson, Lisa M.; Gaskell, M. Gareth – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Recent studies of adults have found evidence for consolidation effects in the acquisition of novel words, but little is known about whether such effects are found developmentally. In two experiments, we familiarized children with novel nonwords (e.g., "biscal") and tested their recognition and recall of these items. In Experiment 1, 7-year-olds…
Descriptors: Evidence, Language Acquisition, Recall (Psychology), Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kamawar, Deepthi; Olson, David R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
To address the question of whether young children are differentially sensitive to referential opacity, an advanced Theory of Mind skill, we assessed 4-, 6-, and 8-year-olds on three types of opaque contexts: epistemic, quotational, and intentional. Children's performance improved as a function of age and varied significantly by opacity type.…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Cognitive Development, Children, Evaluation Methods
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schlottmann, Anne; Ray, Elizabeth D.; Surian, Luca – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Two experiments (N=136) studied how 4- to 6-month-olds perceive a simple schematic event, seen as goal-directed action and reaction from 3 years of age. In our causal reaction event, a red square moved toward a blue square, stopping prior to contact. Blue began to move away before red stopped, so that both briefly moved simultaneously at a…
Descriptors: Evidence, Motion, Habituation, Geometric Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Buss, Aaron T.; Spencer, John P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task requires children to switch from sorting cards based on shape or color to sorting based on the other dimension. Typically, 3-year-olds perseverate, whereas 4-year-olds flexibly sort by different dimensions. Zelazo and colleagues (1996, Cognitive Development, 11, 37-63) asked children questions about the…
Descriptors: Cues, Games, Behavior Standards, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Forman, Helen; Mantyla, Timo; Carelli, Maria G. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
In this longitudinal study, we examined time keeping in relation to working memory (WM) development. School-aged children completed two tasks of WM updating and a time monitoring task in which they indicated the passing of time every 5 min while watching a film. Children completed these tasks first when they were 8 to 12 years old and then 4 years…
Descriptors: Early Adolescents, Children, Short Term Memory, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McAuley, Tara; White, Desiree A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
This study addressed three related aims: (a) to replicate and extend previous work regarding the nonunitary nature of processing speed, response inhibition, and working memory during development; (b) to quantify the rate at which processing speed, response inhibition, and working memory develop and the extent to which the development of these…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Short Term Memory, Psychometrics, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bullens, Jessie; Igloi, Kinga; Berthoz, Alain; Postma, Albert; Rondi-Reig, Laure – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Navigation in a complex environment can rely on the use of different spatial strategies. We have focused on the employment of "allocentric" (i.e., encoding interrelationships among environmental cues, movements, and the location of the goal) and "sequential egocentric" (i.e., sequences of body turns associated with specific choice points)…
Descriptors: Navigation, Spatial Ability, Children, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Simmering, Vanessa R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The change detection task has been used in dozens of studies with adults to measure visual working memory capacity. Two studies have recently tested children in this task, suggesting a gradual increase in capacity from 5 years to adulthood. These results contrast with findings from an infant looking paradigm suggesting that capacity reaches…
Descriptors: Evidence, Infants, Program Effectiveness, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sigelman, Carol K. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
In an examination guided by cognitive developmental and attribution theory of how explanations of wealth and poverty and perceptions of rich and poor people change with age and are interrelated, 6-, 10-, and 14-year-olds (N = 88) were asked for their causal attributions and trait judgments concerning a rich man and a poor man. First graders, like…
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Poverty, Grade 1, Grade 9
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Imbo, Ineke; De Brauwer, Jolien; Fias, Wim; Gevers, Wim – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
In a recent study, Gevers and colleagues (2010, "Journal of Experimental Psychology: General," Vol. 139, pp. 180-190) showed that the SNARC (spatial numerical association of response codes) effect in adults results not only from spatial coding of magnitude (e.g., mental number line hypothesis) but also from verbal coding. Because children are…
Descriptors: Evidence, Experimental Psychology, Number Concepts, Numeracy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perlman, Susan B.; Pelphrey, Kevin A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The regulation of affective arousal is a critical aspect of children's social and cognitive development. However, few studies have examined the brain mechanisms involved in the development of this aspect of "hot" executive functioning. This process has been conceptualized as involving prefrontal control of the amygdala. Here, using functional…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cognitive Development, Affective Behavior, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mills, Candice M.; Legare, Christine H.; Grant, Meridith G.; Landrum, Asheley R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
To obtain reliable information, it is important to identify and effectively question knowledgeable informants. Two experiments examined how age and the ease of distinguishing between reliable and unreliable sources influence children's ability to effectively question those sources to solve problems. A sample of 3- to 5-year-olds was introduced to…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Child Language, Identification, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haden, Catherine A.; Ornstein, Peter A.; O'Brien, Barbara S.; Elischberger, Holger B.; Tyler, Caroline S.; Burchinal, Margaret J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
A multitask battery tapping nonverbal memory and language skills was used to assess 60 children at 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Analyses focused on the degree to which language, working memory, and deliberate memory skills were linked concurrently to children's Elicited Imitation task performance and whether the patterns of association varied…
Descriptors: Imitation, Short Term Memory, Language Skills, Child Development
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  12