NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)0
Since 2006 (last 20 years)69
Source
Journal of Experimental Child…332
Audience
Researchers23
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 332 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rat-Fischer, Lauriane; O'Regan, J. Kevin; Fagard, Jacqueline – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Despite a growing interest in the question of tool-use development in infants, no study so far has systematically investigated how learning to use a tool to retrieve an out-of-reach object progresses with age. This was the first aim of this study, in which 60 infants, aged 14, 16, 18, 20, and 22 months, were presented with an attractive toy and a…
Descriptors: Infants, Toys, Observational Learning, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Perlman, Susan B.; Pelphrey, Kevin A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The regulation of affective arousal is a critical aspect of children's social and cognitive development. However, few studies have examined the brain mechanisms involved in the development of this aspect of "hot" executive functioning. This process has been conceptualized as involving prefrontal control of the amygdala. Here, using functional…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cognitive Development, Affective Behavior, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rafetseder, Eva; Schwitalla, Maria; Perner, Josef – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
The objective of this study was to describe the developmental progression of counterfactual reasoning from childhood to adulthood. In contrast to the traditional view, it was recently reported by Rafetseder and colleagues that even a majority of 6-year-old children do not engage in counterfactual reasoning when asked counterfactual questions…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Individual Development, Children, Preadolescents
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Simmering, Vanessa R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The change detection task has been used in dozens of studies with adults to measure visual working memory capacity. Two studies have recently tested children in this task, suggesting a gradual increase in capacity from 5 years to adulthood. These results contrast with findings from an infant looking paradigm suggesting that capacity reaches…
Descriptors: Evidence, Infants, Program Effectiveness, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sigelman, Carol K. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
In an examination guided by cognitive developmental and attribution theory of how explanations of wealth and poverty and perceptions of rich and poor people change with age and are interrelated, 6-, 10-, and 14-year-olds (N = 88) were asked for their causal attributions and trait judgments concerning a rich man and a poor man. First graders, like…
Descriptors: Attribution Theory, Poverty, Grade 1, Grade 9
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bauer, Patricia J.; Doydum, Ayzit O.; Pathman, Thanujeni; Larkina, Marina; Guler, O. Evren; Burch, Melissa – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Episodic memory is defined as the ability to recall specific past events located in a particular time and place. Over the preschool and into the school years, there are clear developmental changes in memory for when events took place. In contrast, little is known about developmental changes in memory for where events were experienced. In the…
Descriptors: Memory, Recall (Psychology), Geographic Location, Experience
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fu, Genyue; Evans, Angela D.; Xu, Fen; Lee, Kang – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This study investigated whether young children make strategic decisions about whether to lie to conceal a transgression based on the lie recipient's knowledge. In Experiment 1, 168 3- to 5-year-olds were asked not to peek at the toy in the experimenter's absence, and the majority of children peeked. Children were questioned about their…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Strategic Planning, Experiments, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mills, Candice M.; Legare, Christine H.; Grant, Meridith G.; Landrum, Asheley R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
To obtain reliable information, it is important to identify and effectively question knowledgeable informants. Two experiments examined how age and the ease of distinguishing between reliable and unreliable sources influence children's ability to effectively question those sources to solve problems. A sample of 3- to 5-year-olds was introduced to…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Child Language, Identification, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haden, Catherine A.; Ornstein, Peter A.; O'Brien, Barbara S.; Elischberger, Holger B.; Tyler, Caroline S.; Burchinal, Margaret J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
A multitask battery tapping nonverbal memory and language skills was used to assess 60 children at 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Analyses focused on the degree to which language, working memory, and deliberate memory skills were linked concurrently to children's Elicited Imitation task performance and whether the patterns of association varied…
Descriptors: Imitation, Short Term Memory, Language Skills, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gao, Xiaoqing; Maurer, Daphne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Using 20 levels of intensity, we measured children's thresholds to discriminate the six basic emotional expressions from neutral and their misidentification rates. Combined with the results of a previous study using the same method ("Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102" (2009) 503-521), the results indicate that by 5 years of age,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Development, Emotional Response, Nonverbal Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bullens, Jessie; Igloi, Kinga; Berthoz, Alain; Postma, Albert; Rondi-Reig, Laure – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Navigation in a complex environment can rely on the use of different spatial strategies. We have focused on the employment of "allocentric" (i.e., encoding interrelationships among environmental cues, movements, and the location of the goal) and "sequential egocentric" (i.e., sequences of body turns associated with specific choice points)…
Descriptors: Navigation, Spatial Ability, Children, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rodriguez, Purificacion; Lago, M. Oliva; Enesco, Ileana; Guerrero, Silvia – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
In this study, the development of comprehension of essential and nonessential aspects of counting is examined in children ranging from 5 to 8 years of age. Essential aspects, such as logical rules, and nonessential aspects, including conventional rules, were studied. To address this, we created a computer program in which children watched counting…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Computer Software, Computation, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mondloch, Catherine J.; Horner, Matthew; Mian, Jasmine – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Adults' and 8-year-old children's perception of emotional faces is disrupted when faces are presented in the context of incongruent body postures (e.g., when a sad face is displayed on a fearful body) if the two emotions are highly similar (e.g., sad/fear) but not if they are highly dissimilar (e.g., sad/happy). The current research investigated…
Descriptors: Fear, Cognitive Development, Human Posture, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haddad, Jeffrey M.; Claxton, Laura J.; Keen, Rachel; Berthier, Neil E.; Riccio, Gary E.; Hamill, Joseph; Van Emmerik, Richard E. A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Studies have suggested that proper postural control is essential for the development of reaching. However, little research has examined the development of the coordination between posture and manual control throughout childhood. We investigated the coordination between posture and manual control in children (7- and 10-year-olds) and adults during…
Descriptors: Human Posture, Psychomotor Skills, Child Development, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Powell, Nina L.; Derbyshire, Stuart W. G.; Guttentag, Robert E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Two experiments examined biases in children's (5/6- and 7/8-year-olds) and adults' moral judgments. Participants at all ages judged that it was worse to produce harm when harm occurred (a) through action rather than inaction (omission bias), (b) when physical contact with the victim was involved (physical contact principle), and (c) when the harm…
Descriptors: Value Judgment, Cognitive Ability, Moral Development, Moral Values
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  ...  |  23