NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 14 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Steegen, Sara; Neys, Wim De – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Adult reasoning has been shown as mediated by the inhibition of intuitive beliefs that are in conflict with logic. The current study introduces a classic procedure from the memory field to investigate belief inhibition in 12- to 17-year-old reasoners. A lexical decision task was used to probe the memory accessibility of beliefs that were cued…
Descriptors: Evidence, Conflict, Inhibition, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carroll, Daniel J.; Riggs, Kevin J.; Apperly, Ian A.; Graham, Kate; Geoghegan, Ceara – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
A total of 69 preschool children were tested on measures of false belief understanding (the Unexpected Transfer task), inhibitory control (the Grass/Snow task), and strategic reasoning (the Windows task). For each task, children indicated their response either by pointing with their index finger or by using a nonstandard response mode (pointing…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Preschool Children, Inhibition, Feedback (Response)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Drayton, Stefane; Turley-Ames, Kandi J.; Guajardo, Nicole R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The purpose of the current study was to examine further the relationship between counterfactual thinking and false belief (FB) as examined by Guajardo and Turley-Ames ("Cognitive Development, 19" (2004) 53-80). More specifically, the current research examined the importance of working memory and inhibitory control in understanding the relationship…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Development, Beliefs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Liberman, Nira; Polack, Orli; Hameiri, Boaz; Blumenfeld, Maayan – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
According to construal level theory, psychological distance promotes more abstract thought. Theories of creativity, in turn, suggest that abstract thought promotes creativity. Based on these lines of theorizing, we predicted that spatial distancing would enhance creative performance in elementary school children. To test this prediction, we primed…
Descriptors: Priming, Elementary School Students, Creativity, Creativity Tests
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Richland, Lindsey Engle; Chan, Tsz-Kit; Morrison, Robert G.; Au, Terry Kit-Fong – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
A cross-cultural comparison between U.S. and Hong Kong preschoolers examined factors responsible for young children's analogical reasoning errors. On a scene analogy task, both groups had adequate prerequisite knowledge of the key relations, were the same age, and showed similar baseline performance, yet Chinese children outperformed U.S. children…
Descriptors: Cross Cultural Studies, Semantics, Young Children, Cultural Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Merriman, William E.; Lipko, Amanda R.; Evey, Julie A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
Word familiarity judgment may be important for word learning, yet little is known about how children make this judgment. We hypothesized that preschool-age children differ in the judgment criteria that they use and that this difference derives from individual differences in basic memory processes. Those who have superior phonological working…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Vocabulary Development, Word Recognition, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
McCormack, Teresa; Hoerl, Christoph – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
Four studies are reported that employed an object location task to assess temporal-causal reasoning. In Experiments 1-3, successfully locating the object required a retrospective consideration of the order in which two events had occurred. In Experiment 1, 5- but not 4-year-olds were successful; 4-year-olds also failed to perform at above-chance…
Descriptors: Young Children, Time Perspective, Thinking Skills, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carroll, Daniel J.; Apperly, Ian A.; Riggs, Kevin J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
In the present experiment, we used a reversed-contingency paradigm (the windows task: [Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The windows task as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. "British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9," 331-349]) to explore the effect of alterations in the task…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Inhibition, Metacognition, Thinking Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morsanyi, Kinga; Handley, Simon J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2008
We examined the relationship between cognitive capacity and heuristic responding on four types of reasoning and decision-making tasks. A total of 84 children, between 5 years 2 months and 11 years 7 months of age, participated in the study. There was a marked increase in heuristic responding with age that was related to increases in cognitive…
Descriptors: Heuristics, Memory, Young Children, Cognitive Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Chen, Zhe – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1996
Two experiments with children five and eight years of age examined the effects of different types of similarity on analogical problem solving and explored the cognitive components responsible for these effects. Results indicated that superficial and structural similarity facilitated the process of drawing analogies. (WJC)
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Cognitive Measurement, Cognitive Processes, Thinking Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Foltz, Carol; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1995
Studied 100 adolescents' approaches to problem-solving proofs and reasoning competence tasks. Found that a formal level of reasoning competence is associated with a deductive approach. Results support the notion of a cognitive development progression from an inductive approach to a deductive approach. (ETB)
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Adolescents, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Muller, Ulrich; Sokol, Bryan; Overton, Willis F. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1999
Investigated the emergence of class and propositional reasoning skills as a function of the developing ability to coordinate increasingly complex negation and affirmation operations with children from grades 1, 3, 5, and 7. Found that children's reasoning follows a logical development sequence and that different groups of items account for…
Descriptors: Child Development, Children, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Measurement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Greene, Terry R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1991
Second, fourth, and sixth graders were given passage of text whose material could be represented as four-level class inclusion hierarchy. Students were asked to construct external representation of passage and answer questions that required them to reason about contents of passage. Quality of representation and performance on question tasks were…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Galotti, Kathleen M.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Examines development of the concept of commitment in relation to adolescents' choice of a career and romantic partner. Subjects were 163 male and female ninth graders, twelfth graders, and college juniors from public and private schools. Findings support the proposition that the concept of commitment is affected by grade, gender, and type of…
Descriptors: Adolescent Development, Age Differences, Career Choice, Cognitive Development