Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2 |
Descriptor
Difficulty Level | 2 |
Testing | 2 |
Athletics | 1 |
Cognitive Processes | 1 |
College Students | 1 |
Comparative Analysis | 1 |
Correlation | 1 |
Expertise | 1 |
Foreign Countries | 1 |
Imagination | 1 |
Intervals | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
Author
Aïmen Khacharem | 1 |
Bachir Zoudji | 1 |
DiBattista, David | 1 |
Fortuna, Glenda | 1 |
Hajer Mguidich | 1 |
Sinnige-Egger, Jo-Anne | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 2 |
Reports - Research | 2 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Hajer Mguidich; Bachir Zoudji; Aïmen Khacharem – Journal of Experimental Education, 2025
The imagination effect occurs when learners who imagine a procedure perform better on a subsequent test than learners who study it. The present study explored whether this effect is restricted to short-term learning or whether it also applies when learning is tested after a delay. Forty novices and forty experts learned about a basketball game…
Descriptors: Imagination, Expertise, Cognitive Processes, Difficulty Level
DiBattista, David; Sinnige-Egger, Jo-Anne; Fortuna, Glenda – Journal of Experimental Education, 2014
The authors assessed the effects of using "none of the above" as an option in a 40-item, general-knowledge multiple-choice test administered to undergraduate students. Examinees who selected "none of the above" were given an incentive to write the correct answer to the question posed. Using "none of the above" as the…
Descriptors: Multiple Choice Tests, Testing, Undergraduate Students, Test Items