NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 6 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Siegler, Isabelle A.; Bardy, Benoit G.; Warren, William H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
The simple task of bouncing a ball on a racket offers a model system for studying how human actors exploit the physics and information of the environment to control their behavior. Previous work shows that people take advantage of a passively stable solution for ball bouncing but can also use perceptual information to actively stabilize bouncing.…
Descriptors: Physics, Thinking Skills, Task Analysis, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Adolph, Karen E.; Joh, Amy S.; Eppler, Marion A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Three experiments investigated whether 14- and 15-month-old infants use information for both friction and slant for prospective control of locomotion down slopes. In Experiment 1, high- and low-friction conditions were interleaved on a range of shallow and steep slopes. In Experiment 2, friction conditions were blocked. In Experiment 3, the…
Descriptors: Infants, Experimental Psychology, Investigations, Identification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hutchinson, Claire V.; Ledgeway, Tim – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
This study investigated the effect of temporal frequency and modulation depth on reaction times for discriminating the direction of first-order (luminance-defined) and second-order (contrast-defined) motion, equated for visibility using equal multiples of direction-discrimination threshold. Results showed that reaction times were heavily…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Motion, Feedback (Response), Investigations
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Boker, Steven M.; Cohn, Jeffrey F.; Theobald, Barry-John; Matthews, Iain; Mangini, Michael; Spies, Jeffrey R.; Ambadar, Zara; Brick, Timothy R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
During conversation, women tend to nod their heads more frequently and more vigorously than men. An individual speaking with a woman tends to nod his or her head more than when speaking with a man. Is this due to social expectation or due to coupled motion dynamics between the speakers? We present a novel methodology that allows us to randomly…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Speech Communication, Motion, Sexual Identity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bootsma, Reinoud J.; Fernandez, Laure; Morice, Antoine H. P.; Montagne, Gilles – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Using a two-step approach, Van Soest et al. (2010) recently questioned the pertinence of the conclusions drawn by Bootsma and Van Wieringen (1990) with respect to the visual regulation of an exemplary rapid interceptive action: the attacking forehand drive in table tennis. In the first step, they experimentally compared the movement behaviors of…
Descriptors: Architecture, Racquet Sports, Human Body, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Collier, Geoffrey L.; Ogden, R. Todd – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2004
The Wing-Kristofferson model (A. M. Wing & A. B. Kristofferson, (1973a, 1973b) decomposes the variance of isochronous finger tapping into 2 components: a central clock component and a peripheral motor component. The method assumes that there is no drift in the intertap intervals. A new method is introduced that further decomposes the clock…
Descriptors: Intervals, Psychological Studies, Evaluation Methods, Time