Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 20 |
Descriptor
Perception | 21 |
Spatial Ability | 19 |
Experiments | 9 |
Undergraduate Students | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 6 |
Visual Stimuli | 6 |
Attention | 5 |
Experimental Psychology | 4 |
Motion | 4 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 21 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 21 |
Reports - Research | 19 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 10 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (London) | 2 |
Alabama | 1 |
Florida | 1 |
Indiana | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
Israel | 1 |
Kansas | 1 |
Maine | 1 |
Oklahoma | 1 |
South Carolina | 1 |
Texas | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Edinburgh Handedness Inventory | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meiran, Nachshon; Dimov, Eduard; Ganel, Tzvi – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
In the present experiments, the question being addressed was whether switching attention between perceptual dimensions and selective attention to dimensions are processes that compete over a common resource? Attention to perceptual dimensions is usually studied by requiring participants to ignore a never-relevant dimension. Selection failure…
Descriptors: Attention, Perception, Cognitive Ability, Cues
Witt, Jessica K.; Sugovic, Mila; Taylor, J. Eric T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
According to the action-specific account of perception, perceivers see the environment relative to their ability to perform the intended action. For example, in a modified version of the computer game Pong, balls that were easier to block looked to be moving slower than balls that were more difficult to block (Witt & Sugovic, 2010). It is unknown,…
Descriptors: Perception, Ability, Influences, Observation
Tapia, Evelina; Breitmeyer, Bruno G.; Jacob, Jane; Broyles, Elizabeth C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
Flanker congruency effects were measured in a masked flanker task to assess the properties of spatial attention during conscious and nonconscious processing of form, color, and conjunctions of these features. We found that (1) consciously and nonconsciously processed colored shape distractors (i.e., flankers) produce flanker congruency effects;…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes, Perception, Spatial Ability
Turvey, M. T.; Harrison, Steven J.; Frank, Till D.; Carello, Claudia – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Bipedal gaits have been classified on the basis of the group symmetry of the minimal network of identical differential equations (alias "cells") required to model them. Primary gaits are characterized by dihedral symmetry, whereas secondary gaits are characterized by a lower, cyclic symmetry. This fact was used in a test of human…
Descriptors: Perception, Spatial Ability, Experiments, Animals
Davoli, Christopher C.; Brockmole, James R.; Witt, Jessica K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Reaching for an object with a tool has been shown to cause a compressed perception of space just beyond arm's reach. It is not known, however, whether tools that have distal, detached effects at far distances can cause this same perceptual distortion. We examined this issue in the current study with targets placed up to 30m away. Participants who…
Descriptors: Lasers, Memory, Intention, Perception
Caparos, Serge; Linnell, Karina J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Selective attention has been hypothesized to reduce distractor interference at both perceptual and postperceptual levels (Lavie, 2005), respectively, by focusing perceptual resources on the attended location and by blocking at postperceptual levels distractors that survive perceptual selection. This study measured the impact of load on these…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Profiles
Longo, Matthew R.; Haggard, Patrick – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Primary somatosensory maps in the brain represent the body as a discontinuous, fragmented set of two-dimensional (2-D) skin regions. We nevertheless experience our body as a coherent three-dimensional (3-D) volumetric object. The links between these different aspects of body representation, however, remain poorly understood. Perceiving the body's…
Descriptors: Self Concept, Human Body, Cognitive Mapping, Perception
Huang, Liqiang – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
When paying attention to a feature (e.g., red), no attentional advantage is gained in perceiving items with this feature in very brief displays. Therefore, feature-based attention seems to be slow. In previous feature-based attention studies, attention has often been measured as the difference in performance in a secondary task. In our recent work…
Descriptors: Experiments, Stimuli, Attention, Spatial Ability
Linnell, Karina J.; Caparos, Serge – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Caparos and Linnell (2009, 2010) used a variable-separation flanker paradigm to show that (a) when cognitive load is low, increasing perceptual load causes spatial attention to focus and (b) when perceptual load is high, decreasing cognitive load causes spatial attention to focus. Here, we tested whether the effects of perceptual and cognitive…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Attention Control, Attention, Cognitive Processes
Gottesman, Carmela V. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Four experiments examined whether scene processing is facilitated by layout representation, including layout that was not perceived but could be predicted based on a previous partial view (boundary extension). In a priming paradigm (after Sanocki, 2003), participants judged objects' distances in photographs. In Experiment 1, full scenes (target),…
Descriptors: Priming, Experimental Psychology, Universities, Spatial Ability
Witt, Jessica K.; Proffitt, Dennis R.; Epstein, William – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
This research was designed to test the predictions of 2 approaches to perception. By most traditional accounts, people are thought to derive general-purpose spatial perceptions that are scaled in arbitrary, unspecified units. In contrast, action-specific approaches propose that the angular information inherent in optic flow and ocular-motor…
Descriptors: Prediction, Spatial Ability, Perception, Influences
Hock, Howard S.; Nichols, David F. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
A version of the line motion illusion (LMI) occurs when one of two adjacent surfaces changes in luminance; a new surface is perceived sliding in front of the initially presented surface. Previous research has implicated high-level mechanisms that can create or modulate LMI motion via feedback to lower-level motion detectors. It is shown here that…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Perception, Visual Stimuli
Catmur, Caroline; Heyes, Cecilia – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Imitative compatibility, or automatic imitation, has been used as a measure of imitative performance and as a behavioral index of the functioning of the human mirror system (e.g., Brass, Bekkering, Wohlschlager, & Prinz, 2000; Heyes, Bird, Johnson, & Haggard, 2005; Kilner, Paulignan, & Blakemore, 2003). However, the use of imitative…
Descriptors: Evidence, Science Education, Imitation, Spatial Ability
Roberts, Katherine L.; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Perception and action are influenced by the "possibilities for action" in the environment. Neuropsychological studies (e.g., Riddoch, Humphreys, Edwards, Baker, & Willson, 2003) have demonstrated that objects that are perceived to be interacting (e.g., a corkscrew going toward the top of a wine bottle) are perceptually integrated…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Patients, Perception, Experiential Learning
Schwarz, Wolf; Eiselt, Anne-Kathrin – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2009
R. Sekuler, P. Tynan, and E. Levinson (1973) found that when 2 characters are presented side-by-side with a short onset asynchrony, subjectively they often appear in a "first-left, then-right" order. The authors of this article conducted 6 experiments in which observers judged the temporal order (TOJs) in which 2 digits were presented. They found…
Descriptors: Perception, Spatial Ability, Time Perspective, Number Concepts
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2